Skip to main content

Abstract

Background:

There is increasing evidence that long-term exposure to fine particulate matter [PM 2.5μm in aerodynamic diameter (PM2.5)] may adversely impact cognitive performance. Wildfire smoke is one of the biggest sources of PM2.5 and concentrations are likely to increase under climate change. However, little is known about how short-term exposure impacts cognitive function.

Objectives:

We aimed to evaluate the associations between daily and subdaily (hourly) PM2.5 and wildfire smoke exposure and cognitive performance in adults.

Methods:

Scores from 20 plays of an attention-oriented brain-training game were obtained for 10,228 adults in the United States (U.S.). We estimated daily and hourly PM2.5 exposure through a data fusion of observations from multiple monitoring networks. Daily smoke exposure in the western U.S. was obtained from satellite-derived estimates of smoke plume density. We used a longitudinal repeated measures design with linear mixed effects models to test for associations between short-term exposure and attention score. Results were also stratified by age, gender, user behavior, and region.

Results:

Daily and subdaily PM2.5 were negatively associated with attention score. A 10μg/m3 increase in PM2.5 in the 3 h prior to gameplay was associated with a 21.0 [95% confidence interval (CI): 3.3, 38.7]-point decrease in score. PM2.5 exposure over 20 plays accounted for an estimated average 3.7% (95% CI: 0.7%, 6.7%) reduction in final score. Associations were more pronounced in the wildfire-impacted western U.S. Medium and heavy smoke density were also negatively associated with score. Heavy smoke density the day prior to gameplay was associated with a 117.0 (95% CI: 1.7, 232.3)-point decrease in score relative to no smoke. Although differences between subgroups were not statistically significant, associations were most pronounced for younger (18–29 y), older (70y), habitual, and male users.

Discussion:

Our results indicate that PM2.5 and wildfire smoke were associated with reduced attention in adults within hours and days of exposure, but further research is needed to elucidate these relationships. https://doi.org/10.1289/EHP10498

Introduction

Wildfire smoke is a complex mixture of particulate matter (PM) and gases containing many chemical species.1 Both wildfire smoke and many of its components have been previously associated with significant risks to human health (as reviewed by Reid et al.2). Of wildfire emissions, the primary pollutant of public health concern is fine PM [PM 2.5μm in aerodynamic diameter (PM2.5)] (as reviewed by U.S. EPA3). In addition to the extensive body of epidemiologic evidence that daily and subdaily (hourly level)4 exposure to PM2.5 and smoke increases the risk of all-cause mortality and cardiovascular and respiratory morbidity (as reviewed by Atkinson et al.5 and Jaffe et al.1), newer epidemiologic research has found that PM2.5 exposure may also adversely impact cognitive function (as reviewed by Clifford et al.,6 Delgado-Saborit et al.,7 and Xu et al.8). The aim of this study was to further investigate the associations between exposure to PM2.5 and wildfire smoke and cognitive performance. This is a pressing research question because exposure to PM2.5 is ubiquitous, often elevated during wildfire events,1 and likely to increase with the frequency and intensity of wildfires under climate change.912
Current epidemiologic research has identified PM2.5 as a risk factor that can impair cognitive function, accelerate cognitive decline, and increase rates of dementia and Alzheimer’s, especially in younger and older populations (as reviewed by Clifford et al.,6 Delgado-Saborit et al.,7 and Power et al.13). In addition to the epidemiologic literature, there is a growing body of research from animal and human studies that identifies the biological pathways and mechanisms by which air pollution likely impacts neurological health and cognitive function. It shows that PM2.5 can cause systematic inflammation, reach the brain via the olfactory nerve, pass through the blood–brain barrier, or modulate the nervous system, leading to brain inflammation and oxidative stress (as reviewed by Delgado-Saborit et al.,7 Schikowski and Altuğ,14 and U.S. EPA3). Although epidemiologic evidence of the relationships between long-term exposure to PM2.5 and cognitive function is mounting, evidence for short-term exposure is still scarce. The small number of studies that focused on short-term associations have found PM2.5 exposure at the monthly, weekly, and daily level to be associated with increased risk of hospital admissions for neurological disorders1518 and poorer performance on cognitive tests measuring attention, memory, and fluid reasoning, as well as language, math, and reading skills.1925 However, there is limited epidemiologic evidence of the associations with subdaily exposure26 and the associations in the working-age population,23 and, to our knowledge, no evidence of the subdaily associations across multiple age groups or at lower level concentrations, which is largely due to limited availability of data on health outcomes and cognitive performance at the hourly timescale.
Although PM2.5 is a main component of wildfire smoke, the particles have a different composition from typical ambient PM2.5, and smoke also contains toxic chemicals and gases, which may lead to differential health effects compared with typical ambient PM2.5.27,28 Previous research has found agricultural fires and open fire usage indoors to be associated with reduced performance on neurocognitive tests.2931 Although these studies indicate a potentially important association with biomass smoke, to our knowledge, no study to date has investigated the relationship between wildfire smoke exposure and cognitive function. Because concentrations of PM2.5 and smoke can change rapidly over days and hours during wildfire events and given that wildfires pose a growing threat to air quality and public health,1 research is needed to assess how daily and subdaily exposure to PM2.5 and smoke impact cognitive performance and the populations most at risk.
To address these knowledge gaps, we evaluated the associations between short-term PM2.5 and wildfire smoke exposure and cognitive function in a large adult cohort across the contiguous U.S. We measured cognitive performance using data from the Lumosity brain-training platform, which consists of games aimed to measure and improve areas of cognitive function, including memory, attention, flexibility, processing speed, and problem solving. We used a longitudinal repeated measures study design to characterize associations between scores in an attention-oriented Lumosity game, and daily and subdaily PM2.5 and wildfire smoke exposures. We evaluated these associations by age, gender, user behavior, and region to identify vulnerable populations. Although many research efforts have identified serious cardiopulmonary effects of short-term PM2.5 and wildfire smoke exposure, neurological impacts can be more challenging to study because the time between exposure and outcome is often undefined. To our knowledge, this is one of the first epidemiologic studies to use Lumosity data to investigate the associations between short-term air pollution exposure and cognitive performance. It is also the first to investigate these relationships at both the daily and hourly timescale in a large, longitudinal cohort of adults 18 years of age.

Methods

Study Population and Cognitive Outcome

To evaluate the associations between short-term exposure to PM2.5 and wildfire smoke and cognitive performance, we conducted a retrospective analysis of scores in an attention-oriented game on the Lumosity brain-training platform. Specifically, we used user performance data from the game Lost in Migration, which is based on the Flanker Task and designed to target selective attention, the ability to focus on relevant information while ignoring distractions, and response inhibition, the ability to suppress response to distractions.32
In Lost in Migration, the user is shown a flock of five flying birds oriented left, right, up, or down. The goal of the game is for the user to use the arrow keys on a keyboard to correctly identify the orientation of the central bird, which is flanked by four birds oriented in the same or a different direction (Figure S1), as many times as possible. Each time the user selects the orientation of the central bird, correct or incorrect, the formation and orientation of the birds displayed updates. Each game of Lost in Migration lasts 45 s and the number of formations displayed depends on the user’s response speed. Upon completion of the game, users are given an overall score based on their speed, accuracy, and bonus points.33 Each correct answer is worth 50 points multiplied by the bonus at the time the answer was provided. The multiplier bonus is determined by the number of correct answers given in a row and changes over the course of the game. Although incorrect answers do not cause a user to lose points, they can decrease the multiplier bonus. At the end of the game, a user is given bonus points based on their current multiplier bonus. The cognitive focus and use of Lost in Migration as an attentional task has been characterized,34 and the use of Lumosity games, including Lost in Migration, has been shown to improve attention in adults as measured by established neuropsychological tests.3538 In addition, Lost in Migration scores have been used in previous studies as a measure of attention.23,39
The cohort included all Lumosity users in the contiguous U.S. who were 18 years of age and who signed up for the platform and completed between 1 and 20 plays of Lost in Migration during 1 January 2017 to 31 December 2018. The cohort included both free users and paid subscribers. Although users can play Lost in Migration an unlimited number of times, we only had data on the first 20 plays, during which users experience a learning phase where their scores improve at first and then plateau (Figure 1). We restricted analysis to users who completed exactly 20 plays across unique dates (e.g., each play must have occurred on a different day from all other plays) to maximize the number of repeat measures per user and identify daily level associations. To ensure the same study population was used for all analyses, we applied this user inclusion criteria to both the daily and subdaily analyses. In addition, by limiting the analyses to users with 20 plays across unique dates, we were able to exclude users with gaps in play or abnormal playing patterns. We evaluated the robustness of the results with respect to the user inclusion criteria in sensitivity analyses.
Figure 1. (A) Average learning curve for all contiguous U.S. Lumosity users (black, dashed line) and of 100 randomly selected users (gray, solid line); (B) location of the Lumosity users by ZIP3 (dotted areas indicate regions with no users); (C) total number of smoke days (light, medium or heavy) by ZIP3 in the western U.S., 2017–2018; and (D) average population-weighted daily PM2.5 by ZIP3 in the contiguous U.S., 2017–2018. Note: PM2.5, fine particulate matter; ZIP3, first three digits of a ZIP code.
For each Lumosity user, we had data on the first three digits of the user’s ZIP code (ZIP3) location at sign-up, their self-reported age (18y), gender, and education level at sign-up, as well as the device on which all games were played (iPhone, iPad, Android, or web). For each gameplay, data included the user’s score at the end of the game (raw score) and their score relative to a normative population of Lumosity users sampled to match the 2010 U.S. Census for age, gender, and education level (percentile score). We used the raw score (hereafter referred to as the attention score) as the primary measure of cognitive performance and considered the percentile score in a sensitivity analysis. We interpreted the attention score as an indicator of users’ ability to pay attention, given the game’s design. The data set also included the timestamp of each gameplay and the play number, the number of times the user had played Lost in Migration up to and including the current gameplay.

PM2.5 Exposure Data

To estimate ambient PM2.5 for each ZIP3, we used measurements from the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA)’s Federal Reference Method/Federal Equivalent Method (FRM/FEM) monitors and the network of low-cost PurpleAir monitors. Hourly and daily average FRM/FEM observations were downloaded from the U.S. EPA’s Air Quality System database.40 We averaged nonzero FRM/FEM observations at duplicate space/time locations and only kept nonzero daily averages from stations that had more than 75% of hourly observations available for a given day. We used the PurpleAir application programming interface (API)41 to download PurpleAir observations at 80-s or 2-min intervals and calculated daily and hourly averages, following a previously developed quality control and cleaning protocol to ensure quality and completeness.42 PurpleAir sensors are known to overestimate PM2.5 concentrations in both ambient and wildfire conditions.42 To address this, we bias-corrected the PurpleAir data using a modified version of an existing U.S.-wide correction that uses a multiple linear regression with an additive relative humidity term to adjust the observations.42
We used the Bayesian Maximum Entropy (BME) framework to fuse the FRM/FEM and PurpleAir observations and generate daily and hourly estimates of average PM2.5 concentrations at census tract population centers in the contiguous U.S. for 2017–2018. BME is a spatiotemporal modeling framework that can accurately estimate PM2.5 at unmonitored locations by combining information from a site-specific knowledge base (S-KB), which contains information on the observed concentrations at monitoring sites, and a general knowledge base (G-KB), which contains information on the trends and variability in the data.4345 In our implementation of BME, the S-KB included the PM2.5 observations from both the FRM/FEM and PurpleAir stations and the G-KB included the mean and covariance of the observations from the FRM/FEM stations. The PurpleAir data was not used to derive the G-KB given the potential bias of these observations. The S-KB differentiates data with no associated uncertainty (hard data) from data with associated uncertainty (soft data). Hard data have the greatest influence on the BME estimate and the influence of a given hard data observation decreases with increased distance based on the covariance in the G-KB. The influence of the soft data depends on the uncertainty of a given observation, where observations with lower associated uncertainty have greater influence. In our data fusion, we treated the FRM/FEM observations as hard and the PurpleAir observations as soft, using the 95% confidence interval (CI) produced during the PurpleAir bias correction to determine the associated uncertainty. The leave-one-out and 10-fold cross-validation R2 values for the data fusion estimates were 0.815 and 0.795, respectively. The data fusion estimates were generated using MATLAB (version R2017b; MathWorks). BME has previously been used to estimate PM2.5 concentrations across the U.S.4650 and to generate exposure estimates for epidemiologic studies.51,52 Additional details on the theory and mathematical implementation of the BME framework can be found in previously published literature.4350,53
To match the spatial resolution of the Lumosity data, we calculated ZIP3-level population-weighted daily and hourly average PM2.5 concentrations from the census tract-level estimates using the following equation:
PMZIP3j,t=sZIP3jPMs,t×PopssZIP3jPops,
(1)
where PMZIP3j,t is the population-weighted PM2.5 concentration in ZIP3 j on day or hour t; s is a census tract within ZIP3 j; PMs,t is the average PM2.5 data fusion estimate at the population center of census tract s on day or hour t; and Pops is the total population of census tract s, obtained from the 2010 U.S. Census.

Smoke Exposure Data

Wildfire smoke exposure in each ZIP3 in the western U.S. was estimated using smoke density data from the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA)’s Hazard Mapping System (HMS) Fire and Smoke Product.54 The HMS Smoke Product is based on a visual classification of plumes, relying on Advanced Baseline Imager true-color imagery from Geostationary Operational Environmental Satellites (GOES) satellites to identify smoke plumes and a deterministic air quality dispersion model to classify smoke plume density. The density of a plume can be classified as light (PM2.5 concentrations 010μg/m3), medium (PM2.5 concentrations 1021μg/m3), or heavy (PM2.5 concentrations >21μg/m3). HMS smoke plume densities have been used in previous epidemiologic studies to inform exposure55,56 and have been shown to correlate well with surface concentrations.57,58 To match the spatial resolution of the Lumosity data, we calculated the daily maximum smoke density (none, light, medium, or heavy) in each ZIP3, based on all plumes observed over a ZIP3 on a given day. We considered only smoke density data in the western U.S.—defined as Oregon, Washington, California, Montana, Idaho, and Nevada—because that region experiences the largest and most direct wildfire smoke impacts1,59 and we wanted to reduce the potential for visual misclassification of plumes. During 2017 and 2018, there were nearly 33,000 wildfires in these western states, burning close to 11 million acres and accounting for almost 60% of the total acreage burnt by wildfires in the U.S. over these 2 y.60

Statistical Analyses

The primary aim of our analyses was to estimate the associations between the PM2.5 and smoke exposure metrics and the attention scores. To do so, we used a longitudinal repeated measures study design with linear mixed effects models. For daily PM2.5 exposure, we used an unconstrained distributed lag model with 7 lags of population-weighted daily average PM2.5, where lag 0 is the concentration on the day of gameplay, lag 1 is concentration the day prior, and so on. We selected 7 lags to capture associations with same-day exposure24,25 and exposure in the days leading up to play.21 For subdaily PM2.5 exposure, we used the maximum population-weighted hourly average PM2.5 concentration in the 3, 6, and 12 h prior to gameplay. For smoke exposure, we evaluated how daily maximum smoke density on the day of gameplay (lag 0), the day prior to play (lag 1), and in the 1 wk prior to play were associated with attention score. We could not evaluate subdaily exposure to smoke because the HMS data are limited by satellite overpass times. For the PM2.5 analyses, we evaluated the associations between daily and subdaily concentrations and attention scores across the contiguous U.S. To characterize the exposure–outcome relationship in regions directly impacted by wildfire smoke, we also restricted analysis to the western U.S. We conducted the smoke analyses solely in the western U.S., as discussed above.
The secondary aim of our analyses was to explore differences in associations with respect to gender, age group, and user’s habitual behavior to identify particularly vulnerable populations. Users were stratified into six different age groups: 18–29, 30–39, 40–49, 50–59, 60–69, and 70y. Habitual players were defined as users whose median time between plays was 7  d and whose standard deviation (SD) for the time-of-day played was 2  h. Subgroup analyses were conducted in both the western and contiguous U.S. for PM2.5 and solely in the western U.S. for smoke density.
All linear mixed effects models included a log(n) term to control for learning, where n corresponds to the play number. This controls for the learning curve over 20 plays, which followed a logarithmic shape (Figure 1). To further account for improvements in score over time, all models also included a third-order autoregressive process. In addition, the models included an interaction term between log(n), the device type, and the user’s age group to account for the age and device-variable learning curves (Figure S2). Finally, the models included a random intercept by user. The linear mixed effects model used for all analyses can be described using the following equation:
Scoren,i,s,t=β0+u0,i+β1log(n)+β2Scorei,n1+β3Scorei,n2+β4Scorei,n3+β5Exposures,t+β6Devicei+β7AgeGroupi+β8log(n)Devicei+β9log(n)AgeGroupi+β10log(n)AgeGroupiDevicei+βxcovariaten,i,s,t+εn,i,s,t
(2)
where Scoren,i,s,t is user i’s attention score on play n in ZIP3 s at time t. β0 is the intercept; u0,i is the user-specific intercept; and Scorei,n1,2,3 is the third-order autoregression. Exposures,t is the ZIP3-level exposure metric, either PM2.5 concentration or wildfire smoke density, described in detail above; log(n), Devicei, and AgeGroupi are the log of the play number, device, and user age group, respectively. The other covariate terms (covariaten,i,s,t) in the models included: a) categorical variables for the time-of-day (0–23), day-of-week (0–6), and month of the gameplay (1–12), b) the number of days since the user last played, c) user gender and education level, d) ZIP3-level population-weighted daily average relative humidity and temperature, e) ZIP3-level population-weighted annual average PM2.5, and f) the ZIP3’s poverty rate, high school graduation rate, and rural–urban continuum code (RUCC). Temperature and relative humidity data were obtained from NOAA61 and the poverty rate, high school graduation rate, and RUCC were obtained from the U.S. Census Bureau’s 2015–2019 5-y American Community Survey.62 Finally, εn,i,s,t is the error term. By controlling for individual-level characteristics and including a third-order autoregressive process and random intercept by user, we controlled for differences in performance across users and focused the analyses on within-individual differences in responses. Results are reported as a change in attention score associated with a 10μg/m3 increase in daily or subdaily PM2.5 or a change in score associated with light, medium, or heavy density smoke at the daily or weekly level, relative to no smoke. Results were considered statistically significant if the 95% CI of the estimates did not contain 0. Differences between subgroups were considered statistically significant if the 95% CIs of two estimates did not overlap.
Multiple sensitivity analyses were run to assess the robustness of our findings. Specifically, we evaluated the sensitivity of the results to a) using the percentile score as the outcome of interest instead of the raw score, b) including <7 lags in the daily PM2.5 distributed lag model, c) different definitions of habitual users (e.g., different cutoffs for the median time between plays or the SD for time-of-day played), d) less strict inclusion criteria for users (e.g., including users with <20 plays or users who completed 20 plays across <20 unique dates), and e) covariate model specification (e.g., models with less covariate or interaction terms). We also considered the possibility of a nonlinear relationship between daily PM2.5 and attention score by fitting cubic splines to the PM2.5 lags. All statistical analyses were conducted in R (version 4.0.3; R Development Core Team). All data included in the Lumosity data set were deidentified and analyzed in accordance with Lumos Lab, Inc.’s Privacy Policy.63 This study was determined by the Office of Human Research Ethics at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill to not constitute human subjects research as defined under federal regulations and did not require Institutional Review Board approval (reference no. 273020).

Results

Characteristics of Lumosity Users

The study population included 10,228 contiguous U.S. users and 1,809 western U.S. users (Table 1 and Figure 1). The western and contiguous U.S. users had very similar characteristics. Most users were female and 50 years of age. Approximately two-thirds of users had some college education or higher and most played on mobile devices. More than 90% of users lived in metropolitan areas across the U.S. Older users were more likely to be habitual players and use an iPad or the web to play, whereas younger users were less habitual and more likely to use a mobile device. Although there was notable variety in each user’s individual learning curve, the learning curves generally followed a logarithmic curve, with a steeper improvement in attention score during earlier plays and less steep improvement during later plays. On average, users played a game of Lost in Migration every 1–2 wk and improved their score by more than 4,500 points (47.7%) from the 1st to the 20th play. In addition, users completed their plays most frequently in the morning between 0700 and 1200 hours or in the evening between 2000 and 2200 hours (Figure S3). Younger users tended to perform better than older users, with higher attention scores across all 20 plays and steeper learning curves during earlier plays (Figure S4). Web and iPhone users had higher scores in comparison with Android and iPad users, especially among younger players (Figure S2). There were no notable differences in scores or learning curve by gender or habitual behavior (Figure S4). The attention scores across all 20 plays followed a relatively normal distribution (Figure S5). Additional information on the characteristics, learning curves, and attention score distributions of the western and contiguous U.S. study populations, overall and by different user subgroups, can be viewed online at https://ehs-bccdc.shinyapps.io/PMSmoke_Attention_Dashboard/.
Table 1 Characteristics of western and contiguous U.S. Lumosity users and exposure data.
CharacteristicWestern U.S.
(n=1,809)
Contiguous U.S.
(n=10,228)
Gender [n (%)]
 Female1,250 (69.1)7,214 (70.5)
 Male559 (30.9)3,014 (29.5)
Age group (y), [n (%)]
 18–29147 (8.1)859 (8.4)
 30–39254 (12.0)1,238 (12.1)
 40–49276 (15.3)1,530 (15.0)
 50–59457 (25.3)2,752 (26.9)
 60–69427 (23.6)2,614 (25.6)
70248 (13.7)1,235 (12.1)
Education [n (%)]
 Some high school34 (1.9)152 (1.5)
 High school diploma203 (11.2)1,447 (14.1)
 Some college375 (20.7)1,959 (19.2)
 Associate degree178 (9.8)937 (9.2)
 Professional degree91 (5.0)419 (4.1)
 Bachelor’s degree576 (31.8)3,115 (30.5)
 Master’s degree278 (15.4)1,820 (17.8)
 Doctoral degree29 (1.6)190 (1.9)
 Other45 (2.5)189 (1.8)
Device [n (%)]
 Android606 (33.5)3,462 (33.8)
 iPad264 (14.6)1,638 (16.0)
 iPhone668 (36.9)3,858 (37.7)
 Web271 (15.0)1,270 (12.4)
Habitual behavior [n (%)]
 Habitual146 (8.1)873 (8.5)
 Nonhabitual1,663 (91.9)9,355 (91.5)
Attention score [mean (SD)]
 All 20 plays13,161.8 (4,202.5)13,075.5 (4,108.7)
 1st play9,721.5 (4,189.3)9,645.7 (4,093.6)
 20th play14,317.2 (3,928.0)14,250.7 (3,795.7)
 Days between plays [mean (SD)]8.4 (15.1)8.3 (14.0)
 Hour of day played [mean (SD)]13.8 (5.6)13.7 (5.6)
 Daily PM2.5 (μg/m3), [mean (IQR)]a10.0 (6.2)8.7 (5.0)
 Hourly PM2.5 (μg/m3), [mean (IQR)]a10.2 (6.2)9.3 (5.2)
Smoke Density [n (%)]b
 None29,512 (81.6)
 Light3,859 (10.7)
 Medium1,318 (3.6)
 Heavy1,491 (4.1)
Note: There were no missing values in the Lumosity and exposure data sets. —, not applicable; IQR, interquartile range; PM2.5, fine particulate matter; SD, standard deviation; ZIP3, first three digits of a ZIP code.
a
Average of daily and hourly ZIP3-level population-weighted PM2.5 concentrations on the day or hour of gameplay across 36,180 western U.S. observations (1,809 users with 20 plays) and 204,560 contiguous U.S. observations (10,228 users with 20 plays).
b
Total number and percentage of the 36,180 western U.S. observations (1,809 users with 20 plays) with smoke present on the day of gameplay.

Characteristics of Exposure Data

When compared with all users, western U.S. users were exposed to higher levels (mean±SD) of PM2.5 on the day (10.0±6.2 vs. 8.7±5.0μg/m3) or hour (10.2±6.2 vs. 9.3±5.2μg/m3) of gameplay, as well as a wider range of concentrations during the 2017–2018 study period (Table 1). The western states, especially central California and southern Oregon, also had the highest estimated concentrations (Figure 1) and spatial and temporal variability (Figure S6) of PM2.5, in part due to the air quality impacts of wildfires in these regions. In addition, every ZIP3 in the western U.S. had multiple days affected by wildfire smoke during the study period. Southern Oregon and central and northern California were the most smoke impacted during 2017–2018, with smoke plumes identified on nearly 40% of days. Of the 36,180 western U.S. observations included in this study (20 plays for 1,809 users), 18.4% of observations had light, medium, or heavy smoke density present at the ZIP3-level on the day of gameplay, with light smoke density occurring most frequently. Maps of the ZIP3-level population-weighted daily average PM2.5 and daily maximum smoke density, along with maps of the BME data fusion estimates, monitoring station locations, and daily smoke plume shapes, can be viewed online at https://ehs-bccdc.shinyapps.io/PMSmoke_Attention_Dashboard/.

PM2.5 and Cognitive Performance

Daily and subdaily PM2.5 exposure were associated with significant decreases in attention score for both western and contiguous U.S. users, with more pronounced associations in the western states (Figure 2; Table S1). Of the PM2.5 exposure metrics considered, the 7-d cumulative exposure had the strongest association with score. A 10μg/m3 increase in PM2.5 was associated with a 47.6 (95% CI: 7.2, 88.1)- and 33.0 (95% CI: 3.4, 62.5)-point reduction in score in the western and contiguous U.S., respectively. The observed 7-d cumulative associations were driven by the associations with PM2.5 on the day of gameplay (lag 0), which was 44.3 (95% CI: 84.3,4.3) in the western U.S. and 26.4 (95% CI: 47.9,4.9) in the contiguous U.S. The associations at lags 1–6 were largely null. Subdaily exposure was also significantly associated with decreases in attention score. A 10μg/m3 increase in the maximum hourly PM2.5 concentration in the 3 and 12 h prior to gameplay were associated with a 41.7 (95% CI: 13.6, 69.7)- and 40.7 (95% CI: 15.0, 66.4)-point decrease in score, respectively, for western U.S. users. For contiguous U.S. users, the 3- and 12-h maximum exposure metrics were associated with a 21.0 (95% CI: 3.3, 38.7)- and 24.5 (95% CI: 8.3, 40.7)-point decrease in score, respectively.
Figure 2. Change in attention score associated with a 10μg/m3 increase in daily and subdaily PM2.5 for all users in the western and contiguous U.S. Exposure metrics include the maximum population-weighted hourly average PM2.5 in the 3, 6, and 12 h prior to gameplay (3-, 6-, and 12-Hour Max) and the population-weighted daily average PM2.5 in the 7 d prior to gameplay (lags 0–6 and 7-Day Cumulative). The numeric results can be found in Table S1. Note: Hour Max, hour maximum; PM2.5, fine particulate matter.
Consistent with the results for all users, associations between PM2.5 and attention score were generally more pronounced among western U.S. users in the models stratified by age group, gender, and habitual behavior (Figure 3; Table S2). In both the western and contiguous U.S., the youngest (18–29) and older age groups (50–59 and 70y) had the strongest associations with daily and subdaily PM2.5 exposure. The most pronounced associations were observed for those 70 years of age in the western U.S., who had an 89.7 (95% CI: 1.0, 178.4)-point reduction in score per 10μg/m3 of PM2.5 at lag 0. Although the PM2.5-score associations did not vary notably by gender, the associations were generally more pronounced in habitual users, with the magnitude of association two to three times greater than in nonhabitual players across daily and subdaily exposures in the western U.S. For example, in the western U.S., a 10μg/m3 increase in the maximum hourly PM2.5 concentration in the 12 h prior to play was associated with a 122.5 (95% CI: 38.0, 207.0)-point decrease in score for habitual users and only a 34.2 (95% CI: 7.3, 61.10)-point decrease for nonhabitual users. Although the observed differences between age groups and habitual and nonhabitual players are noteworthy, they were not statistically significant. Differences in the precision of the estimates between the western and contiguous U.S. and within subgroups were largely due to differences in the number of users included in each population. Results for all daily (lags 0–6) and subdaily (3-, 6-, and 12-h maximum) exposure metrics for western and contiguous U.S. users, overall and by age, gender, and habitual behavior, can be viewed online at https://ehs-bccdc.shinyapps.io/PMSmoke_Attention_Dashboard/.
Figure 3. Change in attention score associated with a 10μg/m3 increase in daily and subdaily PM2.5 for western and contiguous U.S. users by (A) age group, (B) gender, and (C) habitual behavior. Exposure metrics include the maximum population-weighted hourly average PM2.5 in the 3 and 12 h prior to gameplay (3- and 12-Hour Max), the population-weighted daily average PM2.5 the day of gameplay (Lag 0), and the cumulative population-weighted daily average PM2.5 in the 7 d prior to gameplay (7-Day Cumulative). The numeric results can be found in Table S2. Note: Hour Max, hourly maximum; PM2.5, fine particulate matter.

Wildfire Smoke and Cognitive Performance

Wildfire smoke density was negatively associated with attention score at both the daily and weekly level among western U.S. users (Figure 4; Table S3). There were significant decreases in attention score when medium density smoke was present the day of gameplay (lag 0) and when heavy density smoke was present the day prior to play (lag 1) and in the 1 wk prior to play. The presence of light density smoke prior to play was not associated with score. The strongest associations were observed for heavy smoke density at lag 1 and in the 1 wk prior to play, with 117.0 (95% CI: 1.7, 232.3)- and 119.3 (95% CI: 26.4, 212.2)-point decreases in score relative to no smoke, respectively. Aligning with the PM2.5 results, the observed associations with heavy smoke density in the 1 wk prior to play were driven by the associations at lag 1.
Figure 4. Change in attention score associated with light, medium, or heavy density smoke, relative to no smoke, for (A) all western U.S. users and by (B) age group, (C) gender, and (D) habitual behavior. Exposure metrics include the daily maximum smoke density the day of and day prior to gameplay (Lag 0 and Lag 1) and in the 1 wk prior to gameplay (1-Week). The numeric results can be found in Table S3.
The age, gender, and habitual behavior-specific associations were consistent with the results for all western U.S. users, where the presence of medium or heavy smoke density had significant negative associations with attention score (Figure 4; Table S3). Although the results by age group were inconsistent across densities and exposure metrics, the associations with heavy smoke were most pronounced for users 18–29 and 40–49 years of age. For example, for users 40–49 years of age, the presence of heavy smoke density in the 1 wk prior to play was associated with a 261.3 (95% CI: 3.1, 519.6)-point reduction in score. In addition, the scores of male users had stronger associations with smoke density than female users, with the most notable differences observed for heavy smoke. For male users, heavy smoke density at lag 1 was associated with a 258.6 (95% CI: 43.0, 474.3)-point decrease in score relative to no smoke, whereas for female users it was associated with only a 52.2 (95% CI: 84.3, 188.8)-point decrease. Further, aligning with the PM2.5 results, the associations with smoke density were generally more pronounced in habitual users than nonhabitual users. The presence of medium smoke density the day of gameplay was associated with a 269.7 (95% CI: 69.8, 609.2)-point decrease for habitual users compared with a 98.8 (95% CI: 15.7, 213.3)-point decrease for nonhabitual users. Although younger, male, and habitual user attention scores appeared to have stronger associations with wildfire smoke, the differences between user subgroups were not statistically significant. Results for all smoke analyses can also be viewed online at https://ehs-bccdc.shinyapps.io/PMSmoke_Attention_Dashboard/.

Sensitivity Analyses

Multiple sensitivity analyses were run to assess the robustness of the methods and findings (Tables S4 and S5). When the percentile score was used as the outcome of interest instead of the raw score, the results were largely consistent with the primary findings, albeit slightly attenuated in the contiguous U.S. (Figure S7). The daily PM2.5 distributed lag model was not sensitive to the number of lags included, with both the lag 0 and n-day cumulative associations remaining consistent regardless of how many lags were used (Figure S8). Further, the results were generally insensitive to the definition of habitual users. When less strict definitions were considered, habitual users still had stronger associations with PM2.5 and smoke density than nonhabitual users, although the differences between the two groups were less pronounced (Figure S9). The results were also robust to covariate model specification, with consistent results when less adjusted models were used (e.g., models with less covariate or interaction terms) (Figure S10). We also investigated the possibility of a nonlinear relationship between PM2.5 and attention score. For both the western and contiguous U.S., the nonlinear relationships had wide CIs and the associations across all concentrations were null (Figure S11).
Additional sensitivity analyses examined how relaxing the inclusion criteria for users affected the observed associations. When the inclusion criteria were relaxed to include users who completed 20 plays across <20 unique dates, the associations between PM2.5 and smoke density and score were slightly attenuated (Figure S12). Similarly, when we relaxed the inclusion criteria to include users who completed <20 plays, the associations across all exposure metrics were attenuated (Figure S13), indicating that the results were somewhat sensitive to user longevity on the platform. Results for all sensitivity analyses can also be viewed online at https://ehs-bccdc.shinyapps.io/PMSmoke_Attention_Dashboard/.

Discussion

We found evidence of significant associations between short-term exposure to PM2.5 and wildfire smoke and decreased attention in adults, as measured by Lumosity’s Lost in Migration game. Exposure to PM2.5 at the daily and hourly level were associated with a reduction of more than 20 points in attention score per 10μg/m3 of PM2.5. Likewise, exposure to heavy or medium smoke density at the daily and weekly level were associated with a reduction of >100 points in attention score relative to no smoke.
Using the estimated association at lag 0, we can compare users’ predicted scores at the observed PM2.5 concentrations with their predicted scores in the absence of PM2.5, concentrations of 0μg/m3. By doing so, we estimate that western U.S. users lost on average a total of 887 (95% CI: 86, 1,688) points over 20 plays, with an average 40- to 50-point loss for each play, due to same-day PM2.5 exposure. The estimated loss of 887 points from the average 20th play score of 14,317 points corresponds to a 6.2% (95% CI: 0.6%, 11.8%) reduction in final score associated with PM2.5. Further, assuming no multiplier bonus is applied, an 887-point loss is equivalent to 18 fewer correct answers over 20 plays. In comparison, we estimate contiguous U.S. users lost on average 529 (95% CI: 98, 960) points [3.7% (95% CI: 0.7%, 6.7%) reduction in 20th play score and 11 fewer correct answers] over 20 plays due to PM2.5.
The strongest associations with PM2.5 were observed within a short exposure window, showing that PM2.5 is associated with reduced attention within 3 h of exposure. Although we were unable to evaluate the impacts of subdaily exposure to wildfire smoke, the strongest associations were also observed within a short exposure window of 2d. Because the PM2.5 associations were driven by hourly and same-day exposure, we hypothesize that the relationships between short-term variations in PM2.5 and cognitive performance, such as those experienced during wildfire smoke episodes, may be transient. However, given that we observed associations at ambient concentrations typically experienced in U.S. communities in the absence of extreme air pollution events, it is likely that long-term exposure also has implications for cognitive function. Such associations have been observed in recent epidemiologic studies identifying the chronic cognitive impacts of sustained exposure,6467 but further research into the links between extended PM2.5 and wildfire smoke exposure and cognitive performance is needed.
In addition to varying by exposure duration, the associations of PM2.5 with attention were also more pronounced in the western U.S., with the magnitude of association nearly double that of the contiguous U.S. This may be due in part to geographic differences in the sources and chemical composition of PM2.5.68 Wildfires are a significant source of PM2.5 in the western U.S., which can affect the toxicity of the particles and may modify their associated health effects.27,28,69 The other physical and mental health effects of wildfire events may also play a role. For example, eye irritation70,71 could impact the ability of users to effectively interact with mobile devices, and increased anxiety72,73 could affect the ability to focus. The observed associations with both PM2.5 and smoke density in the western U.S. provide evidence that individuals living in wildfire-impacted regions may be more vulnerable to the cognitive impacts of air pollution. This vulnerability may be further exacerbated as PM2.5 concentrations in areas affected by wildfires increase under climate change.9,10,74
Further, although the differences were not statistically significant, habitual users had stronger associations with both smoke and PM2.5 exposure than nonhabitual users. This is potentially due to behavioral differences between the groups. Because habitual users train more frequently, their decision to play may be less dependent on environmental conditions and their short-term variations in exposure may be better represented with areal averages when compared with more sporadic users. In addition, the moderate sensitivity of the results to the user inclusion criteria could also be due to differences in user behavior. Prior research has found that Lumosity users who drop out early tend to exhibit poorer performance and shallower learning trajectories, potentially due to lower motivation or less desire to continue playing.75 Although it is not fully clear how lower motivation to play impacts the associations between PM2.5 and smoke and Lumosity performance, the observed attenuation may be due to stronger exposure misclassification in users with less predictable patterns of behavior or the suppression of scores in users with less desire to play. However, all possibilities for the cause of any user behavior-related differences will be unknown until Lumosity data is used more broadly in a research context.
Our findings are largely consistent with existing epidemiologic research. Prior studies have identified associations between daily PM2.5 concentrations and poorer cognitive performance, as measured by Lumosity performance23 and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE),21 global cognitive function,21 fluid reasoning,24 and standardized test scores.19,20,22,25 However, this analysis did not have the power to detect a nonlinear association as was reported by some prior studies.21,23,24 Although there is very limited research on the relationships between cognitive function and subdaily exposure, one study with 63 participants identified an association between hourly exposure to PM via outdoor commuting and candle burning and reduced MMSE performance.26 In addition, although differences between age groups were not statistically significant, younger and older users having stronger associations with exposure aligns with existing research linking PM2.5 to reduced academic and cognitive performance in youth and accelerated cognitive decline in older adults (as reviewed by Clifford et al.6). The only other study that has used Lumosity data to investigate how daily PM2.5 varies with cognitive performance did not report significant associations among older users, but they did find younger users to be more impacted.23 Finally, although no prior epidemiologic study has examined the association between wildfire smoke density and cognitive performance, our results are generally consistent with other studies that identified associations between agricultural fires and open fire usage and decreased cognitive function, as measured by MMSE,29 word recall,2931 and mental intactness30,31 tests.
Our study has many strengths, the first of which is the repeated measures study design. The Lumosity data set provided detailed, longitudinal measurements for a large adult population, enabling us to conduct one of the first large-scale studies of the links between short-term PM2.5 and wildfire smoke exposure and cognitive function. This allowed us to overcome some of the limitations associated with clinical or laboratory studies, which can be time and cost intensive with a limited number of participants. In addition, because the data set provided repeat measures for over 10,000 diverse users across the contiguous U.S., we were able to identify significant associations in the larger working-age population. Another notable strength is our use of BME data fusion to estimate PM2.5 concentrations. Most such studies rely solely on data from the closest monitoring station to inform exposure.1925 By fusing observations from both FRM/FEM and PurpleAir monitors, we were able to increase the spatial coverage of observations and get more information on PM2.5 exposure at the hourly and daily level while still accounting for the uncertainty in the PurpleAir measurements. Further, by population-weighting the PM2.5 estimates to the ZIP3 level, we were able to account for the spatial distribution of the population and give proportionally greater weight to the PM2.5 experienced in the most densely populated regions. This approach likely generated estimates that were more representative of exposure than taking a simple spatial average. Finally, an important strength of this study was the ability to investigate associations with subdaily PM2.5 exposure. Most epidemiologic studies on the health effects of PM2.5 and wildfire smoke have focused on 24-h average exposure (as reviewed by Reid et al.2 and Atkinson et al.5), often because data on health outcomes are only available at the daily timescale.4 By identifying how quickly changes in hourly PM2.5 concentrations are linked to decreased cognitive performance, our findings may be useful for time-sensitive public health decision-making processes during extreme air pollution events, such as wildfires.
This study is not without limitations. First, performance on the Lumosity platform is not a clinical measure of cognitive function. Use of Lumosity has been associated with improved performance in other neuropsychological tests,35,36,7678 and brain-training games can improve performance on tasks involving similar cognitive domains (as reviewed by Simons et al.79 and Smid et al.80). In addition, Lost in Migration scores have been used in other studies as a measure of attention,23,39 and the cognitive focus of Lumosity games has been characterized.34 However, it is unclear whether changes Lumosity scores have generalizable impacts on everyday learning and cognitive tasks. Second, there is the possibility of exposure misclassification. The analyses were conducted at the ZIP3 level, which is a relatively coarse resolution for studying the impacts of PM2.5 and smoke given that concentrations and density can change rapidly over short distances. In addition, we assumed that users did not move and completed all plays at their sign-up ZIP3 location. It is very possible that users, especially those using mobile devices, played Lost in Migration in different ZIP3s during their 20 plays, which would lead to nondifferential exposure misclassification and attenuation of the true associations toward the null. This is especially true in regions with higher variability in PM2.5 concentrations within a ZIP3, such as in the western U.S. (Figure S5). In addition, the visibility on smoke days may influence a user’s movement and in turn their performance or decision to play. Although this is difficult to verify with the current data, we confirmed that 6,668 plays occurred on 409 smoke days in 96 ZIP3s. In comparison, 6,716 plays occurred on the 508 nonsmoke days in the week prior to a smoke day in same ZIP3s. This leads us to believe that a user’s decision to play is likely not influenced by the presence or visibility of smoke. Third, the inclusion of paid subscribers in the cohort did not constitute a random sample. The Lumosity brain-training games are promoted as tools aimed to improve cognitive abilities and it is unclear how the willingness to pay and motivation to play affects the direction and magnitude of the observed associations. This should be considered in future investigations. Fourth, we were unable to control for all individual-level factors, such as socioeconomic status, leaving the possibility of residual confounding. However, we were able to control for related confounders, such as education at the individual level and socioeconomic status at the ZIP3 level. In addition, given the relatively coarse spatial resolution of the Lumosity data set, we may have been unable to account for all spatial correlation present in the data. Further, although the results of the subgroup analyses are informative, showing more pronounced associations in younger (18–29 y), older (70y), and male users, the differences between groups were not statistically significant. There was also a lack of precision for some of the subgroup estimates due to small sample sizes. Additional research using a larger study population is needed to clearly identify vulnerable subpopulations. Finally, the generalizability of our findings may be limited to adults with similar demographics to our study population who use brain-training games, such as Lumosity.
Despite these limitations, our findings help expand the knowledge base on the cognitive function risks posed by poor air quality. This is one of the first epidemiologic studies to identify the link between daily and subdaily PM2.5 exposure and cognitive performance in the working-age population and to show that PM2.5 is associated with reduced attention within hours of exposure. It is also the first to identify an association between wildfire smoke density and decreased cognitive performance. Further, we show that Lumosity data can be a useful tool for investigating the associations between environmental exposures and cognitive function. The platform provides cognitive performance data for a large population using controlled and repeated games that can be combined with appropriate statistics to conduct robust epidemiologic research. Future work could include expanding analyses to Lumosity games beyond Lost in Migration that target cognitive domains other than attention. In addition, research using Lumosity data over a longer duration could enable the investigation of the short- and longer-term impacts of exposure in tandem. Research considering the interaction between wildfire smoke and PM2.5 could also be informative given that PM2.5 is likely on the causal pathway between smoke exposure and cognitive performance and that prior studies have found PM-health associations to be more pronounced on smoky days,81 possibly because of very high concentrations, PM composition, or coexposure with gaseous pollutants. Finally, additional research into the associations between short-term PM2.5 and wildfire smoke exposure and cognitive function using different measures of cognitive performance and in other wildfire-impacted regions would be valuable to validate our findings and further elucidate these relationships.

Acknowledgments

We thank Lumos Labs, Inc., for making the deidentified Lost in Migration data set available for research purposes through the Human Cognition Project and for their continued collaboration and guidance. We also thank A. Patil for her assistance with the fine particulate matter (PM2.5) and meteorological data sets. This project was supported by an appointment (of S.E.C.) to the Research Participation Program at the Center for Public Health and Environmental Assessment, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA), administered by the Oak Ridge Institute for Science and Education through an interagency agreement between the U.S. Department of Energy and the U.S. EPA. The Bayesian Maximum Entropy data fusion estimates of daily and hourly average PM2.5 at census tract population centers can be downloaded at https://doi.org/10.15139/S3/Z9WSWC. The ZIP3 (first three digits of a ZIP code)-level daily maximum smoke density and population-weighted daily and hourly average PM2.5 exposure data can be downloaded at https://doi.org/10.23719/1523337. All results for the primary and sensitivity analyses conducted, as well as maps of the exposure surfaces and additional information on the Lumosity data set, can be viewed online at https://ehs-bccdc.shinyapps.io/PMSmoke_Attention_Dashboard/. Although this work has been reviewed for publication by the U.S. EPA, it does not necessarily reflect the views and policies of the agency. Mention of trade names or commercial products does not constitute endorsement or recommendation for use.

Article Notes

The authors declare they have nothing to disclose.

Supplementary Material

File (ehp10498.smcontents.508.pdf)
File (ehp10498.s001.acco.pdf)

References

1.
Jaffe DA, O’Neill SM, Larkin NK, Holder AL, Peterson DL, Halofsky JE, et al. 2020. Wildfire and prescribed burning impacts on air quality in the United States. J Air Waste Manag Assoc 70(6):583–615. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32240055/, https://doi.org/10.1080/10962247.2020.1749731.
2.
Reid CE, Brauer M, Johnston FH, Jerrett M, Balmes JR, Elliott CT. 2016. Critical review of health impacts of wildfire smoke exposure. Environ Health Perspect 124(9):1334–1343. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27082891/, https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.1409277.
3.
U.S. EPA (U.S. Environmental Protection Agency). 2019. Integrated Science Assessment (ISA) for Particulate Matter (Final Report, Dec 2019). EPA/600/R-19/188. Washington, DC: U.S. EPA.
4.
Yao J, Brauer M, Wei J, McGrail KM, Johnston FH, Henderson SB. 2020. Sub-daily exposure to fine particulate matter and ambulance dispatches during wildfire seasons: a case-crossover study in British Columbia, Canada. Environ Health Perspect 128(6):67006. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32579089/, https://doi.org/10.1289/EHP5792.
5.
Atkinson RW, Kang S, Anderson HR, Mills IC, Walton HA. 2014. Epidemiological time series studies of PM2.5 and daily mortality and hospital admissions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Thorax 69(7):660–665. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24706041/, https://doi.org/10.1136/thoraxjnl-2013-204492.
6.
Clifford A, Lang L, Chen R, Anstey KJ, Seaton A. 2016. Exposure to air pollution and cognitive functioning across the life course—a systematic literature review. Environ Res 147:383–398. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26945620/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2016.01.018.
7.
Delgado-Saborit JM, Guercio V, Gowers AM, Shaddick G, Fox NC, Love S. 2021. A critical review of the epidemiological evidence of effects of air pollution on dementia, cognitive function and cognitive decline in adult population. Sci Total Environ 757:143734. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33340865/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.143734.
8.
Xu X, Ha SU, Basnet R. 2016. A review of epidemiological research on adverse neurological effects of exposure to ambient air pollution. Front Public Health 4:157. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27547751/, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2016.00157.
9.
Boegelsack N, Withey J, O’Sullivan G, McMartin D. 2018. A critical examination of the relationship between wildfires and climate change with consideration of the human impact. J Environ Prot (Irvine, Calif) 9(5):461–467, https://doi.org/10.4236/jep.2018.95028.
10.
Ford B, Martin MV, Zelasky SE, Fischer EV, Anenberg SC, Heald CL, et al. 2018. Future fire impacts on smoke concentrations, visibility, and health in the contiguous United States. Geohealth 2(8):229–247. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32159016/, https://doi.org/10.1029/2018GH000144.
11.
McKenzie D, Littell JS. 2017. Climate change and the eco-hydrology of fire: will area burned increase in a warming western USA. Ecol Appl 27(1):26–36. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28001335/, https://doi.org/10.1002/eap.1420.
12.
Abatzoglou JT, Williams AP. 2016. Impact of anthropogenic climate change on wildfire across western US forests. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113(42):11770–11775. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27791053/, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1607171113.
13.
Power MC, Adar SD, Yanosky JD, Weuve J. 2016. Exposure to air pollution as a potential contributor to cognitive function, cognitive decline, brain imaging, and dementia: a systematic review of epidemiologic research. Neurotoxicology 56:235–253. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27328897/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2016.06.004.
14.
Schikowski T, Altuğ H. 2020. The role of air pollution in cognitive impairment and decline. Neurochem Int 136:104708. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32092328/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuint.2020.104708.
15.
Zanobetti A, Dominici F, Wang Y, Schwartz JD. 2014. A national case-crossover analysis of the short-term effect of PM2.5 on hospitalizations and mortality in subjects with diabetes and neurological disorders. Environ Health 13(1):38. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24886318/, https://doi.org/10.1186/1476-069X-13-38.
16.
Culqui DR, Linares C, Ortiz C, Carmona R, Díaz J. 2017. Association between environmental factors and emergency hospital admissions due to Alzheimer’s disease in Madrid. Sci Total Environ 592:451–457. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28342386/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2017.03.089.
17.
Lee S, Lee W, Kim D, Kim E, Myung W, Kim SY, et al. 2019. Short-term PM2.5 exposure and emergency hospital admissions for mental disease. Environ Res 171:313–320. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30711732/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2019.01.036.
18.
Wu Z, Chen X, Li G, Tian L, Wang Z, Xiong X, et al. 2020. Attributable risk and economic cost of hospital admissions for mental disorders due to PM2.5 in Beijing. Sci Total Environ 718:137274. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32109812/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.137274.
19.
Heissel J, Persico C, Simon D. 2019. Does pollution drive achievement? The effect of traffic pollution on academic performance. NBER Working Paper Series. Working paper 25489. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w25489.
20.
Lavy V, Ebenstein A, Roth S. 2014. The impact of short term exposure to ambient air pollution on cognitive performance and human capital formation. NBER Working Paper Series. Working paper 20648. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w20648.
21.
Gao X, Coull B, Lin X, Vokonas P, Spiro A III, Hou L, et al. 2021. Short-term air pollution, cognitive performance and nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drug use in the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study. Nat Aging 1(5):430–437. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/34841262/, https://doi.org/10.1038/s43587-021-00060-4.
22.
Amanzadeh N, Vesal M, Ardestani SFF. 2020. The impact of short-term exposure to ambient air pollution on test scores in Iran. Popul Environ 41(3):253–285, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11111-019-00335-4.
23.
La Nauze A, Severnini ER. 2021. Air pollution and adult cognition: evidence from brain training. NBER Working Paper Series. Working paper 28785. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/W287850.
24.
Bedi AS, Nakaguma MY, Restrepo BJ, Rieger M. 2021. Particle pollution and cognition: evidence from sensitive cognitive tests in Brazil. J Assoc Environ Resour Econ 8(3):443–474, https://doi.org/10.1086/711592.
25.
Ebenstein A, Lavy V, Roth S. 2016. The long-run economic consequences of high-stakes examinations: evidence from transitory variation in pollution. Am Econ J Appl Econ 8(4):36–65, https://doi.org/10.1257/app.20150213.
26.
Shehab MA, Pope FD. 2019. Effects of short-term exposure to particulate matter air pollution on cognitive performance. Sci Rep 9(1):8237. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31160655/, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44561-0.
27.
Park M, Joo HS, Lee K, Jang M, Kim SD, Kim I, et al. 2018. Differential toxicities of fine particulate matters from various sources. Sci Rep 8(1):17007. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30451941/, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-35398-0.
28.
Aguilera R, Corringham T, Gershunov A, Benmarhnia T. 2021. Wildfire smoke impacts respiratory health more than fine particles from other sources: observational evidence from southern California. Nat Commun 12(1):1493. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33674571/, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-21708-0.
29.
Maher BA, O’Sullivan V, Feeney J, Gonet T, Anne Kenny R. 2021. Indoor particulate air pollution from open fires and the cognitive function of older people. Environ Res 192:110298. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33039528/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2020.110298.
30.
Lai W, Li Y, Tian X, Li S. 2017. Agricultural fires and cognitive function: evidence from crop production cycles. SSRN Electron J. Posted online 21 September 2017, https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3039935.
31.
Lai W, Li S, Li Y, Tian X. 2021. Air pollution and cognitive functions: evidence from straw burning in China. Am J Agric Econ 104(1):190–208, https://doi.org/10.1111/ajae.12225.
32.
Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. 1974. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys 16(1):143–149, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267.
33.
Lumos Labs. 2021. Lost in Migration Instructions. https://help.lumosity.com/hc/en-us/articles/360048973634-Lost-in-Migration-Instructions [accessed 24 January 2022].
34.
Steyvers M, Schafer RJ. 2020. Inferring latent learning factors in large-scale cognitive training data. Nat Hum Behav 4(11):1145–1155. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32868884/, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00935-3.
35.
Ballesteros S, Prieto A, Mayas J, Toril P, Pita C, Ponce de León L, et al. 2014. Brain training with non-action video games enhances aspects of cognition in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Front Aging Neurosci 6:277. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25352805/, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00277.
36.
Al-Thaqib A, Al-Sultan F, Al-Zahrani A, Al-Kahtani F, Al-Regaiey K, Iqbal M, et al. 2018. Brain training games enhance cognitive function in healthy subjects. Med Sci Monit Basic Res 24:63–69. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29674605/, https://doi.org/10.12659/msmbr.909022.
37.
Mayas J, Parmentier FBR, Andrés P, Ballesteros S. 2014. Plasticity of attentional functions in older adults after non-action video game training: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 9(3):e92269. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24647551/, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092269.
38.
Finn M, McDonald S. 2011. Computerised cognitive training for older persons with mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study using a randomised controlled trial design. Brain Impair 12(3):187–199, https://doi.org/10.1375/brim.12.3.187.
39.
Richards A, Kanady JC, Huie JR, Straus LD, Inslicht SS, Levihn-Coon A, et al. 2020. Work by day and sleep by night, do not sleep too little or too much: effects of sleep duration, time of day and circadian synchrony on flanker-task performance in internet brain-game users from teens to advanced age. J Sleep Res 29(6):e12919. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31631467/, https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12919.
40.
U.S. EPA. 2022. Air Quality System (AQS). https://www.epa.gov/aqs [accessed 28 September 2020].
41.
PurpleAir. 2021. API - PurpleAir. https://api.purpleair.com/ [accessed 10 April 2021].
42.
Barkjohn KK, Gantt B, Clements AL. 2021. Development and application of a United States-wide correction for PM2.5 data collected with the PurpleAir sensor. Atmos Meas Tech 4(6):4617–4637. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/34504625/, https://doi.org/10.5194/amt-14-4617-2021.
43.
Christakos G. 1990. A Bayesian/maximum-entropy view to the spatial estimation problem. Math Geol 22(7):763–777, https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00890661.
44.
Christakos G, Bogaert P, Serre ML. 2002. Temporal GIS: Advanced Functions for Field-Based Applications. New York, N.Y.: Springer-Verlag.
45.
Serre ML, Christakos G. 1999. Modern geostatistics: computational BME analysis in the light of uncertain physical knowledge—the Equus Beds study. Stoch Environ Res Risk Assess 13(1–2):1–26, https://doi.org/10.1007/s004770050029.
46.
Reyes JM, Serre ML. 2014. An LUR/BME framework to estimate PM2.5 explained by on road mobile and stationary sources. Environ Sci Technol 48(3):1736–1744. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24387222/, https://doi.org/10.1021/es4040528.
47.
Akita Y, Chen JC, Serre ML. 2012. The moving-window Bayesian maximum entropy framework: estimation of PM2.5 yearly average concentration across the contiguous United States. J Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol 22(5):496–501. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22739679/, https://doi.org/10.1038/jes.2012.57.
48.
Jerrett M, Turner MC, Beckerman BS, Pope CA III, van Donkelaar A, Martin RV, et al. 2017. Comparing the health effects of ambient particulate matter estimated using ground-based versus remote sensing exposure estimates. Environ Health Perspect 125(4):552–559. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27611476/, https://doi.org/10.1289/EHP575.
49.
Beckerman BS, Jerrett M, Serre M, Martin RV, Lee SJ, Van Donkelaar A, et al. 2013. A hybrid approach to estimating national scale spatiotemporal variability of PM2.5 in the contiguous United States. Environ Sci Technol 47(13):7233–7241. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23701364/, https://doi.org/10.1021/es400039u.
50.
Cleland SE, West JJ, Jia Y, Reid S, Raffuse S, O’Neill S, et al. 2020. Estimating wildfire smoke concentrations during the October 2017 California fires through BME space/time data fusion of observed, modeled, and satellite-derived PM2.5. Environ Sci Technol 54(21):13439–13447. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33064454/, https://doi.org/10.1021/acs.est.0c03761.
51.
Chen JC, Wang X, Serre M, Cen S, Franklin M, Espeland M. 2017. Particulate air pollutants, brain structure, and neurocognitive disorders in older women. Res Rep Health Eff Inst 193:1–54. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31898881/.
52.
Younan D, Petkus AJ, Widaman KF, Wang X, Casanova R, Espeland MA, et al. 2020. Particulate matter and episodic memory decline mediated by early neuroanatomic biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 143(1):289–302. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31746986/, https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awz348.
53.
Xu Y, Serre ML, Reyes J, Vizuete W. 2016. Bayesian maximum entropy integration of ozone observations and model predictions: a national application. Environ Sci Technol 50(8):4393–4400. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26998937/, https://doi.org/10.1021/acs.est.6b00096.
54.
National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration. 2020. Hazard Mapping System Fire and Smoke Product. https://www.ospo.noaa.gov/Products/land/hms.html [accessed 11 November 2020].
55.
Jones CG, Rappold AG, Vargo J, Cascio WE, Kharrazi M, McNally B, et al. 2020. Out‐of‐hospital cardiac arrests and wildfire‐related particulate matter during 2015–2017 California wildfires. J Am Heart Assoc 9(8):e014125. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32290746/, https://doi.org/10.1161/JAHA.119.014125.
56.
Wettstein ZS, Hoshiko S, Fahimi J, Harrison RJ, Cascio WE, Rappold AG. 2018. Cardiovascular and cerebrovascular emergency department visits associated with wildfire smoke exposure in California in 2015. J Am Heart Assoc 7(8):e007492. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29643111/, https://doi.org/10.1161/JAHA.117.007492.
57.
Larsen AE, Reich BJ, Ruminski M, Rappold AG. 2018. Impacts of fire smoke plumes on regional air quality, 2006–2013. J Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol 28(4):319–327. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29288254/, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41370-017-0013-x.
58.
Preisler HK, Schweizer D, Cisneros R, Procter T, Ruminski M, Tarnay L. 2015. A statistical model for determining impact of wildland fires on particulate matter (PM2.5) in Central California aided by satellite imagery of smoke. Environ Pollut 205:340–349. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26123723/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envpol.2015.06.018.
59.
Burke M, Driscoll A, Heft-Neal S, Xue J, Burney J, Wara M. 2021. The changing risk and burden of wildfire in the United States. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 118(2):e2011048118. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33431571/, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2011048118.
60.
National Interagency Fire Center. 2021. Fire Information. Statistics. https://www.nifc.gov/fire-information/statistics [accessed 22 July 2021].
61.
National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration. National Centers for Environmental Information (NCEI) Global Surface Summary of the Day - GSOD. https://www.ncei.noaa.gov/metadata/geoportal/rest/metadata/item/gov.noaa.ncdc%3AC00516/html# [accessed 13 October 2021].
62.
U.S. Census Bureau. American Community Survey 5-Year Data (2009–2020). https://www.census.gov/data/developers/data-sets/acs-5year.2019.html [accessed 8 March 2022].
63.
Lumos Labs. 2022. Privacy Policy. https://www.lumosity.com/en/legal/privacy_policy/ [accessed 24 January 2022].
64.
Younan D, Wang X, Casanova R, Barnard R, Gaussoin SA, Saldana S, et al. 2021. PM2.5 associated with gray matter atrophy reflecting increased Alzheimer risk in older women. Neurology 96(8):e1190–e1201. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33208540/, https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000011149.
65.
Kulick ER, Elkind MSV, Boehme AK, Joyce NR, Schupf N, Kaufman JD, et al. 2020. Long-term exposure to ambient air pollution, APOE-ε4 status, and cognitive decline in a cohort of older adults in northern Manhattan. Environ Int 136:105440. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31926436/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2019.105440.
66.
Aretz B, Janssen F, Vonk JM, Heneka MT, Boezen HM, Doblhammer G. 2021. Long-term exposure to fine particulate matter, lung function and cognitive performance: a prospective Dutch cohort study on the underlying routes. Environ Res 201:111533. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/34153335/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2021.111533.
67.
Shi L, Wu X, Danesh Yazdi M, Braun D, Abu Awad Y, Wei Y, et al. 2020. Long-term effects of PM2.5 on neurological disorders in the American Medicare population: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Planet Health 4(12):e557–e565. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33091388/, https://doi.org/10.1016/S2542-5196(20)30227-8.
68.
Liu JC, Peng RD. 2019. The impact of wildfire smoke on compositions of fine particulate matter by ecoregion in the Western US. J Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol 29(6):765–776. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30185941/, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41370-018-0064-7.
69.
Bell ML, Ebisu K, Peng RD, Samet JM, Dominici F. 2009. Hospital admissions and chemical composition of fine particle air pollution. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 179(12):1115–1120. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19299499/, https://doi.org/10.1164/rccm.200808-1240OC.
70.
Viswanathan S, Eria L, Diunugala N, Johnson J, McClean C. 2006. An analysis of effects of San Diego wildfire on ambient air quality. J Air Waste Manag Assoc 56(1):56–67. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16499147/, https://doi.org/10.1080/10473289.2006.10464439.
71.
Mirabelli MC, Künzli N, Avol E, Gilliland FD, Gauderman WJ, McConnell R, et al. 2009. Respiratory symptoms following wildfire smoke exposure: airway size as a susceptibility factor. Epidemiology 20(3):451–459. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19276978/, https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e31819d128d.
72.
Dodd W, Scott P, Howard C, Scott C, Rose C, Cunsolo A, et al. 2018. Lived experience of a record wildfire season in the Northwest Territories, Canada. Can J Public Health 109(3):327–337. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29981098/, https://doi.org/10.17269/s41997-018-0070-5.
73.
Papanikolaou V, Adamis D, Mellon RC, Prodromitis G. 2011. Psychological distress following wildfires disaster in a rural part of Greece: a case-control population-based study. Int J Emerg Ment Health 13(1):11–26. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/21957753/.
74.
Liu JC, Mickley LJ, Sulprizio MP, Dominici F, Yue X, Ebisu K, et al. 2016. Particulate air pollution from wildfires in the western US under climate change. Clim Change 138(3):655–666. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28642628/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-016-1762-6.
75.
Steyvers M, Benjamin AS. 2019. The joint contribution of participation and performance to learning functions: exploring the effects of age in large-scale data sets. Behav Res Methods 51(4):1531–1543. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30251006/, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1128-2.
76.
Ng NF, Osman AM, Kerlan KR, Doraiswamy PM, Schafer RJ. 2021. Computerized cognitive training by healthy older and younger adults: age comparisons of overall efficacy and selective effects on cognition. Front Neurol 11:564317. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33505344/, https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2020.564317.
77.
Hardy JL, Nelson RA, Thomason ME, Sternberg DA, Katovich K, Farzin F, et al. 2015. Enhancing cognitive abilities with comprehensive training: a large, online, randomized, active-controlled trial. PLoS One 10(9):e0134467. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26333022/, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134467.
78.
Kesler S, Hadi Hosseini SM, Heckler C, Janelsins M, Palesh O, Mustian K, et al. 2013. Cognitive training for improving executive function in chemotherapy-treated breast cancer survivors. Clin Breast Cancer 13(4):299–306. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23647804/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clbc.2013.02.004.
79.
Simons DJ, Boot WR, Charness N, Gathercole SE, Chabris CF, Hambrick DZ, et al. 2016. Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol Sci Public Interest 17(3):103–186. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27697851/, https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100616661983.
80.
Smid CR, Karbach J, Steinbeis N. 2020. Toward a science of effective cognitive training. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 29(6):531–537, https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420951599.
81.
Faustini A, Alessandrini ER, Pey J, Perez N, Samoli E, Querol X, et al. 2015. Short-term effects of particulate matter on mortality during forest fires in Southern Europe: results of the MED-PARTICLES project. Occup Environ Med 72(5):323–329. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25691696/, https://doi.org/10.1136/oemed-2014-102459.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Environmental Health Perspectives
Volume 130Issue 6June 2022
PubMed: 35700064

History

Received: 14 October 2021
Revision received: 27 April 2022
Accepted: 2 May 2022
Published online: 14 June 2022

Authors

Affiliations

Department of Environmental Sciences and Engineering, Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
Oak Ridge Institute for Science and Education at the Center for Public Health and Environmental Assessment, Office of Research and Development (ORD), U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA), Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, USA
Center for Public Health and Environmental Assessment, ORD, U.S. EPA, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, USA
Linda Wei
Center for Public Health and Environmental Assessment, ORD, U.S. EPA, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, USA
Environmental Health Services, British Columbia Centre for Disease Control, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
Department of Environmental Sciences and Engineering, Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
Department of Environmental Sciences and Engineering, Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
Environmental Health Services, British Columbia Centre for Disease Control, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
Ana G. Rappold
Center for Public Health and Environmental Assessment, ORD, U.S. EPA, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, USA

Notes

Address correspondence to Ana G. Rappold, U.S. EPA Human Studies Facility, 104 Mason Farm Rd., Chapel Hill, NC 27514 USA. Email: [email protected]

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

About Article Metrics


Citations

Download citation

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click DOWNLOAD.

Cited by

  • Eucalyptus Wood Smoke Extract Elicits a Dose-Dependent Effect in Brain Endothelial Cells, International Journal of Molecular Sciences, 10.3390/ijms251910288, 25, 19, (10288), (2024).
  • Climate Disaster and Cognitive Ability: Evidence From Wildfire, International Journal of Public Health, 10.3389/ijph.2024.1607128, 69, (2024).
  • Parkinson’s Disease Is Predominantly an Environmental Disease, Journal of Parkinson's Disease, 10.3233/JPD-230357, 14, 3, (451-465), (2024).
  • Impact of airborne particulate matter exposure on hospital admission for Alzheimer's disease and the attributable economic burden: evidence from a time-series study in Sichuan, China, Environmental Sciences Europe, 10.1186/s12302-023-00833-1, 36, 1, (2024).
  • Climate Change, Landscape Fires, and Human Health: A Global Perspective, Annual Review of Public Health, 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-060222-034131, 45, 1, (295-314), (2024).
  • Health Effects of Wildfire Smoke Exposure, Annual Review of Medicine, 10.1146/annurev-med-052422-020909, 75, 1, (277-292), (2024).
  • Commercial real estate and air pollution, Real Estate Economics, 10.1111/1540-6229.12484, 52, 3, (951-989), (2024).
  • Robust differential gene expression patterns in the prefrontal cortex of male mice exposed to an occupationally relevant dose of laboratory-generated wildfire smoke, Toxicological Sciences, 10.1093/toxsci/kfae097, 201, 2, (300-310), (2024).
  • Smoke on the horizon: leveling up citizen and social science to motivate health protective responses during wildfires, Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 10.1057/s41599-024-02641-1, 11, 1, (2024).
  • Performance of Vehicle Add-on Mobile Monitoring System PM 2.5 measurements during wildland fire episodes , Environmental Science: Atmospheres, 10.1039/D3EA00170A, 4, 3, (306-320), (2024).
  • See more

View Options

View options

PDF

View PDF

Get Access

Restore your content access

Enter your email address to restore your content access:

Note: This functionality works only for purchases done as a guest. If you already have an account, log in to access the content to which you are entitled.

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Copy the content Link

Share on social media