Skip to main content
Open access
Review
9 September 2010

Lead and PCBs as Risk Factors for Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Publication: Environmental Health Perspectives
Volume 118, Issue 12
Pages 1654 - 1667

Abstract

Objectives

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most frequently diagnosed neurobehavioral disorder of childhood, yet its etiology is not well understood. In this review we present evidence that environmental chemicals, particularly polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) and lead, are associated with deficits in many neurobehavioral functions that are also impaired in ADHD.

Data sources

Human and animal studies of developmental PCB or lead exposures that assessed specific functional domains shown to be impaired in ADHD children were identified via searches of PubMed using “lead” or “PCB exposure” in combination with key words, including “attention,” “working memory,” “response inhibition,” “executive function,” “cognitive function,” “behavior,” and “ADHD.”

Data synthesis

Children and laboratory animals exposed to lead or PCBs show deficits in many aspects of attention and executive function that have been shown to be impaired in children diagnosed with ADHD, including tests of working memory, response inhibition, vigilance, and alertness. Studies conducted to date suggest that lead may reduce both attention and response inhibition, whereas PCBs may impair response inhibition to a greater degree than attention. Low-level lead exposure has been associated with a clinical diagnosis of ADHD in several recent studies. Similar studies of PCBs have not been conducted.

Conclusions

We speculate that exposures to environmental contaminants, including lead and PCBs, may increase the prevalence of ADHD.
With an estimated worldwide prevalence of 5.29% (Polanczyk et al. 2007), attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most common neurobehavioral disorder of childhood. ADHD has an onset at early school age and is characterized by impulsive behavior and inattention, with three main clinical phenotypes: a) predominantly inattentive, b) predominantly hyperactive-impulsive, and c) combined, which includes characteristics of both of the first two phenotypes (American Psychiatric Association 2000).
Aspects of both executive functioning and attention are impaired in ADHD [for a review, see Aguiar et al. (2010)]. Executive function is an interrelated set of cognitive abilities, including working memory, response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and planning, that are involved in goal-oriented problem solving (Marcovitch and Zelazo 2009). Working memory is the ability to keep information in mind momentarily while using the information to perform an action or while performing an unrelated action (Baddeley 1986). Response inhibition is the ability to either inhibit or interrupt a response, with impaired response inhibition manifesting as impulsivity (Winstanley et al. 2006). Cognitive flexibility (or set shifting) is the ability to reallocate mental resources based on a change in situational demands (e.g., when the rules for successfully completing a task change) (Monsell 2003). Attention consists of several components, including alertness, which is the ability to enter an alert state and immediately focus on important aspects of a situation, and vigilance (or sustained attention), which is the ability to maintain the alert state for a period of time (Nigg and Nikolas 2008). Although all of these neurobehavioral functions are compromised in ADHD children to some extent, the evidence suggests that working memory, response inhibition, and vigilance are affected to a greater extent (Aguiar et al. 2010).
Considerable research has been devoted to identifying factors that contribute to ADHD, but we are still a long way from fully understanding its etiology. ADHD is a highly heritable disorder, with pooled data from twin studies suggesting a heritability of 76% (Smith et al. 2009). Yet emerging evidence indicates that many neurodevelopmental disorders, including ADHD, result from complex interactions of genetic, environmental, and social factors (Pennington et al. 2009). Neuroimaging studies suggest that corticostriatal circuitry in brain regions that have key roles in executive function and attention is altered in ADHD, and several converging lines of evidence point to dysfunctional catecholaminergic signaling as the underlying deficit (Vaidya and Stollstorff 2008). Among the contributing factors, the potential role of toxic exposures, especially those that alter catecholaminergic signaling, deserves special scrutiny because these exposures are, in theory, preventable. Much has been written about ADHD, and a number of recent reviews have discussed the role of environmental exposures (e.g., Banerjee et al. 2007; Nigg 2006; Swanson et al. 2007). However, none has provided an in-depth analysis of the parallels between the performance of contaminant-exposed and ADHD children on specific cognitive tests.
In this review, we highlight the parallels between the performance of ADHD children on tests of working memory, response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, planning, and attention [reviewed by Aguiar et al. (2010)] and the performance of children and laboratory animals on similar tasks after developmental exposure to lead or polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs). This synthesis builds on several insightful earlier reviews that first drew attention to these commonalities (Cory-Slechta 2003; Rice 1996, 2000).
Human and animal studies of developmental lead or PCB exposure that assessed specific functional domains shown to be impaired in ADHD children were identified via searches of PubMed (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/) using lead or PCB exposure in combination with the key words “executive function,” “working memory,” “response inhibition,” “attention,” “cognitive function,” “behavior,” and “ADHD,” among others. Because the literature on the effects of lead on cognition in children is so extensive, we included only lead studies published after 1990 that either tested specific neurobehavioral functions relevant to ADHD or directly assessed the relationship between lead exposure and a diagnosis of ADHD. We applied similar criteria to PCB studies, with the exception that we included studies before 1990. For studies of lead in animals, we included those that evaluated effects of developmental exposure on cognitive domains relevant to ADHD and reported steady-state blood lead levels (BLLs) < 100 μg/dL. Findings from animal studies that reported steady state BLLs > 100 μg/dL were not included because they are of questionable relevance to humans. We included PCB studies in animals that evaluated individual noncoplanar congeners or mixtures, but we excluded studies of coplanar PCB congeners because exposure to these congeners results in few cognitive deficits in animal models (Sable and Schantz 2006). Findings were considered statistically significant when defined as such in the original papers, typically with an alpha level of 0.05.

Lead

Epidemiologic research

Numerous studies have demonstrated that lead negatively affects children’s cognitive abilities and behavior, and deficits have been observed at BLLs well below the action level of 10 μg/dL set by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC 2005). The vast majority of the studies have used measures of general intelligence or intelligence quotient (IQ) as the primary dependent variable (Bellinger 2008; Lanphear et al. 2005). However, some studies have evaluated the association between low-level lead exposure and aspects of executive function and attention that are implicated in ADHD (Table 1). Brief definitions of the tests used in these studies and the behavioral domains the tests assess are provided in Supplemental Material, Table 1 (doi:10.1289/ehp.0901852). Many of these tests are also described in more detail in the context of ADHD in the companion review by Aguiar et al. (2010).
Table 1 Childhood lead exposure and performance on tests of functions impaired in ADHD.
Domain/testCohortAge (years)OutcomeaReference
Working memory

 Verbal
  WRAMLNECAT6.0–10.0↓ visual and verbal scoresSurkan et al. 2007
 Detroit 17.5— story memoryChiodo et al. 2004
  Digit spanNECAT6.0–10.0Surkan et al. 2007
 Amsterdam 18.3–12.0Minder et al. 1994
 Amsterdam 29.0–12.0Minder et al. 1998
 Detroit 17.5↓ number correctChiodo et al. 2004
 Korea7.0–16.0↓ number correctMin et al. 2007
  WISC-RBoston10.0↓ freedom from Distractibility scoresStiles and Bellinger 1993
 Mexico7.0— freedom from Distractibility scoresKordas et al. 2006
  WISC-IIIDetroit 17.5— freedom from Distractibility scoresChiodo et al. 2004
  CVLT-CBoston10.0— immediate recall, ↑perseverative responsesStiles and Bellinger 1993
  MemorybCincinnati15.0–17.0— composite memory factorRis et al. 2004
  Story RecallBoston10.0— immediate recallStiles and Bellinger 1993
  SternbergcDetroit 17.5— number correctChiodo et al. 2004
 Amsterdam 29.0–12.0— number correctMinder et al. 1998
 Mexico7.0↓ number correctKordas et al. 2006
 Nonverbal
  CANTAB
   Spatial spanRochester5.0↑nontarget errorsCanfield et al. 2004
   Spatial working memory  ↑total errors 
  Visual memory spanMexico7.0Kordas et al. 2006
  SeashoredDetroit 17.5↓ number correctChiodo et al. 2004
  CorsieDetroit 17.5— number correctChiodo et al. 2004
  Pattern MemoryGermany6.0— number correctWalkowiak et al. 1998

Response inhibition

 CPTDetroit 17.5— commission errorsChiodo et al. 2004
 Detroit 27.0— commission errorsChiodo et al. 2007
 Germany6.0↑commission errorsWalkowiak et al. 1998
 Visual searchMexico7.0— commission errorsKordas et al. 2006
 CRTAmsterdam 18.3–12.0— false responsesMinder et al. 1994
 DRLOswego9.0↓ interresponse timesStewart et al. 2006

Cognitive flexibility

 WCSTBoston10.0↑perseverative responsesStiles and Bellinger 1993
 NECAT6.0–10.0↑perseverative responses
↓ categories completed
Surkan et al. 2007
 Detroit 17.5↑perseverative responses
↓ conceptual level responses
Chiodo et al. 2004
 StroopfAmsterdam 18–12— time to completeMinder et al. 1994
 NECAT6.0–10.0— interference scoreSurkan et al. 2007
 Trails-BgAmsterdam 18.3–12.0↑time to completeMinder et al. 1994
 Amsterdam 29.0–12.0— time to completeMinder et al. 1998
 NECAT6.0–10.0— time to completeSurkan et al. 2007
 CANTAB ID-ED ShiftRochester5.0↓ stages completed
↓ completion of ED shift
Canfield et al. 2004

Planning

 WISC-R mazesAmsterdam 18.3–12.0— correct responsesMinder et al. 1994
 WISC-III mazesNECAT6.0–10.0— correct responsesSurkan et al. 2007
 CANTAB SOCRochester5.0↑no. moves to solve
↑planning time
Canfield et al. 2004
 TOLDetroit 17.5— no. trials to solveChiodo et al. 2004
 ROCFBoston10.0↓ copy organization scoresStiles and Bellinger 1993

Attention

 Vigilance
  CPTDetroit 17.5↓ number correctChiodo et al. 2004
 Detroit 27.0↑omission errorsChiodo et al. 2007
 Germany6.0↑omission errorsWalkowiak et al. 1998
  AttentionhCincinnati15.0–17.0↓ attentionRis et al. 2004
  UnderliningAmsterdam 18.3–12.0— number correctMinder et al. 1994
 Alertness
  CPTDetroit 17.5— reaction timeChiodo et al. 2004
 Detroit 27.0↑reaction timeChiodo et al. 2007
  SternbergDetroit 17.5↑reaction timeChiodo et al. 2004
  Mental rotationDetroit 17.5↑reaction timeChiodo et al. 2004
  SRTTAmsterdam 18.3–12.0↑reaction timeMinder et al. 1994
 Amsterdam 29.0–12.0— reaction timeMinder et al. 1998
 NECAT6.0–10.0— reaction timeSurkan et al. 2007
 Korea7.0–16.0↑reaction timeMin et al. 2007
 Germany6.0— reaction timeWalkowiak et al. 1998
  Stimulus discriminationMexico7.0— reaction timeKordas et al. 2006
Abbreviations: CANTAB, Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery; CPT, continuous performance task; CRT, Choice Reaction Time task; CVLT-C, California Verbal Learning Test for Children; DRL, differential reinforcement of low rates of responding; ID-ED, Intradimensional-Extradimensional Shift test; NECAT, New England Children’s Amalgam Trial; ROCF, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; SOC, Stockings of Cambridge; SRTT, Simple Reaction Time test; TOL, Tower of London; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WISC-R, Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children–Revised; WISC-III, Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children III; WRAML, Wide-Range Assessment of Memory and Learning.
a
↑indicates significant increase associated with lead exposure; ↓ indicates significant decrease; — indicates no association.
b
Memory factor identified by principal component analysis; included short and long delay recall from the CVLT and ROCF.
c
Sternberg Memory test.
d
Seashore Rhythm test.
e
The Corsi test is a visual-spatial analogue of the digit span test.
f
Stroop Color-Word test.
g
Trail Making Test Part B.
h
Attention factor identified by principal component analysis; included CPT omission errors, CPT commission errors and hit reaction time.

Lead and executive function

Working memory

Meta-analyses report impaired performance on both verbal working memory and nonverbal working memory tests in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010). A handful of epidemiologic studies have assessed the relationship between childhood lead exposure and performance on tests of working memory. These include assessments of prospective birth cohorts in Boston, Massachusetts; Rochester, New York; and Cincinnati, Ohio, as well as several cross-sectional studies. Stiles and Bellinger (1993) assessed working memory in a prospective birth cohort study with detailed information on the children’s lifetime lead exposure. A total of 148 children from an original sample of 249 middle- and upper-class children in Boston were assessed at 10 years of age. The mean BLL of these children was < 8 μg/dL at all ages. Scores on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Revised (WISC-R) Freedom from Distractibility Index, which includes arithmetic and digit span and is believed to assess primarily verbal working memory, were inversely associated with BLL at 2 years of age. However, no associations between BLL and immediate recall on either the California Verbal Learning Test for Children (CVLT-C) or the Story Recall test were observed.
Canfield et al. (2003, 2004) examined the association between lifetime average BLL (mean = 7.2; range, 0–20 μg/dL) and performance on tests of spatial working memory in a prospective cohort of 174 children from Rochester, at 4 years of age using the Shape School task and at 5 years of age using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Tests Automated Battery (CANTAB). Few effects were observed at 4 years, but at 5 years children with higher lifetime BLLs showed impaired performance on tests measuring both spatial working memory and spatial memory span. These findings closely parallel the changes seen on the same spatial working memory tasks in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010).
Ris et al. (2004) examined the relationship between childhood BLL and performance on a battery of tests of executive function and attention in 195 children 15–17 years of age from the Cincinnati prospective birth cohort, an inner-city African-American cohort in which 80% of the children had at least one childhood BLL measurement > 15 μg/dL. The investigators used principal components analysis to identify five neuropsychological factors, including a memory factor. However, the memory factor was not related to BLL.
Chiodo et al. (2004) examined the relationship between current BLL and performance on tasks measuring working memory in a cross-sectional study of 237 inner-city African-American children 7.5 years of age from Detroit, Michigan (referred to as Detroit 1 in Table 1). The BLLs of 92% of the children were < 10 μg/dL. BLL was associated with poorer verbal working memory on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–III (WISC-III) Digit Span and poorer nonverbal working memory on the Seashore Rhythm test, although scores on the WISC-III Freedom from Distractibility Index and the Sternberg Memory test were not affected.
The association between childhood lead exposure and verbal working memory was also evaluated in two cohorts of children from the Netherlands. Minder et al. (1994) did not find a relationship between hair lead and performance on the digit span test in a small cross-sectional study of 43 boys 8–12 years of age enrolled in special education (Amsterdam 1 cohort). In a later study, Minder et al. (1998) tested a larger sample of 313 boys 9–12 years of age enrolled in special education (Amsterdam 2 cohort) and did not find any associations between BLL and performance on either digit span or the Sternberg Memory test. BLLs in these children averaged 4.4 μg/dL (range, 0.8–16.0 μg/dL), which is similar to the BLLs reported in other recent studies. However, these are the only studies to assess the impact of lead exposure in children enrolled in special education. Detecting an additional impact of low-level lead exposure could be more difficult in children with learning disabilities.
A small study of 61 Korean children 7–16 years of age also evaluated the relationship between current BLL and performance on a digit span test (Min et al. 2007). Lead exposure was very low in this cohort, with an average BLL of only 2.9 μg/dL, yet decreased digit span scores indicative of impaired verbal working memory were observed.
Surkan et al. (2007) compared 6- and 10-year-old children whose current BLLs were 5–10 μg/dL with children whose BLLs were 1–2 μg/dL in the New England Children’s Amalgam Trial (NECAT) (n = 389). They found that children with higher BLLs had poorer verbal working memory as assessed on the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning (WRAML).
Kordas et al. (2006) assessed the relationship between current BLL and several measures of working memory in 602 first-graders (average BLL, 11.4 μg/dL) from Mexico. They observed a significant negative association only for number correct on the Sternberg Memory test among the ADHD-relevant memory tests they administered. In summary, although some epidemiologic studies of lead exposure have reported an inverse relationship between BLL and performance on working memory tests, unlike ADHD, the findings are somewhat inconsistent across studies and across individual tests within studies.

Response inhibition

Impaired response inhibition is another hallmark of ADHD (Aguiar et al. 2010), and the effects of lead on response inhibition have been evaluated in several studies. Walkowiak et al. (1998) evaluated a sample of 384 6-year-old German children on a continuous performance task (CPT) from the Neurobehavioral Evaluation System 2. Current BLL (average, 4.25 μg/dL) was associated with increases in errors of commission (responding to nontarget stimuli), a pattern similar to that exhibited by ADHD children on CPTs (Aguiar et al. 2010). However, BLL was not associated with increased commission errors in either the Detroit 1 cohort (Chiodo et al. 2004) or a second (Detroit 2) cohort of 467 children 7 years of age with current BLLs that averaged 5 μg/dL (Chiodo et al. 2007). In the 2007 study, BLL also was not associated with impulsivity on any of three behavioral rating scales: the Conners Teacher Ratings Scale, Achenbach’s Teacher Report Form, and PROBS-14 Problem Behavior Scale.
Stewart et al. (2006) used an operant schedule frequently used in animal models [differential reinforcement of low rates of responding (DRL)] to evaluate the association between childhood BLL and response inhibition in 167 children from Oswego, New York. They found that BLLs at 2–4 years of age (mean, 4.58 μg/dL) were associated with excessive responding on the DRL task at 9.5 years of age, indicative of impaired response inhibition. These findings resemble those seen in ADHD children tested on a similar schedule, the fixed interval (FI) task (Sagvolden et al. 1998), and in an animal model for ADHD, the spontaneously hypertensive rat, when tested on the FI task (Sagvolden 2000).

Cognitive flexibility and planning

The impact of lead exposure on cognitive flexibility has been assessed in a number of studies, most often using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). Increased perseverative responding (persistent, incorrect responding after the response rule has changed) on the WCST was associated with increased BLL in all three studies that used this test (Chiodo et al. 2004; Stiles and Bellinger 1993; Surkan et al. 2007). This parallels the increase in perseverative responding typically observed in ADHD children on the WCST (Aguiar et al. 2010). Poorer performance on the CANTAB Intradimensional–Extradimensional Shift test, a computerized analogue of the WCST, also has been associated with increased BLL (Canfield et al. 2004). However, the results using other tests of cognitive flexibility have not been as consistent. Two lead studies (Amsterdam 1 and NECAT) found no association between BLL and scores on the Stroop Color-Word test (Minder et al. 1994; Surkan et al. 2007), and only one of three studies (Amsterdam 1, but not Amsterdam 2 or NECAT) found an association between BLL and scores on the Trail Making Test Part B (Minder et al. 1994, 1998; Surkan et al. 2007). Interestingly, the performance of ADHD children on the Stroop Color-Word test also is not as clearly or consistently impaired as their performance on the WCST (Aguiar et al. 2010).
Planning is another cognitive domain that is impaired in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010). Although elevated BLL has been associated with deficits in planning in some studies, the results have not been consistent across studies. Increased BLL was associated with increased planning time and a greater number of moves to solve the CANTAB Stockings of Cambridge test, a computerized analogue of the Tower of London (TOL) task, in the Rochester lead cohort (Canfield et al. 2004). However, in the Detroit 1 cohort BLL did not appear to affect performance on the TOL task (Chiodo et al. 2004). This latter finding is in contrast to the impaired performance of ADHD children on the TOL task (Aguiar et al. 2010). Poorer performance on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCF) was associated with higher BLL in the Boston cohort (Stiles and Bellinger 1993), which is consistent with the findings in ADHD children on this task (Aguiar et al. 2010). The ability to solve mazes was not affected by lead exposure in the Amsterdam 1 (Minder et al. 1994) and NECAT (Surkan et al. 2007) cohorts.

Lead and attention

Vigilance and alertness are two aspects of attention that are impaired in ADHD children, and both appear to be affected by lead exposure. When CPTs were used to assess attention, impaired vigilance, marked by a decrease in the number of correct responses and an increase in omission errors, was associated with BLL in both of the Detroit cohorts (Chiodo et al. 2004, 2007) and in the German cohort (Walkowiak et al. 1998). In contrast, longer reaction times on the CPT, a measure of alertness, were associated with increased BLL in the Detroit 2 but not the Detroit 1 cohort (Chiodo et al. 2004, 2007). Both findings, increased omission errors and increased reaction times, are observed when ADHD children are tested on CPTs (Aguiar et al. 2010). Additionally, for children in the Detroit 2 cohort, BLL was associated with attentional problems on the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (Chiodo et al. 2007). Higher BLL also was associated with higher ADHD and inattention scores on the Barkley–DuPaul ADHD Scale, poorer attention on the Achenbach Checklist, and higher ratings of withdrawn and off-task behaviors on the Barkley Direct Observation Scale for children in the Detroit 2 cohort (Chiodo et al. 2004).
Deficits in alertness are also suggested by associations of BLL or hair lead with longer reaction times while performing several other tests, including simple reaction time tests, the Sternberg Memory task, a mental rotation task, and a stimulus discrimination test in the Detroit 1 (Chiodo et al. 2004), the Amsterdam 1 (Minder et al. 1994), and the Korean (Min et al. 2007) cohorts, but not in the Amsterdam 2 (Minder et al. 1998), NECAT (Surkan et al. 2007), German (Walkowiak et al. 1998), or Mexican (Kordas et al. 2006) cohorts. Finally, Ris et al. (2004) identified a significant association between childhood BLLs and an attention factor identified by principal components analysis, which included CPT omission errors (vigilance) and reaction time (alertness). A stronger association of BLL with attentional problems was observed in boys than in girls. This does not appear to be attributable to differences in exposure because there does not seem to be a sex-related difference in childhood BLL in the United States based on 1988–2004 data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) (Jones et al. 2009). Other epidemiologic studies of lead exposure generally have not reported whether there are sex differences in outcomes, but it is well established that boys are at greater risk for ADHD (Aguiar et al. 2010).
In summary, studies of lead exposure in children provide evidence for impairments in several aspects of executive function and attention that are impaired in ADHD children. The functions most consistently affected across studies include cognitive flexibility, vigilance, and alertness. There is also some evidence that impairments in working memory, planning, and response inhibition are associated with lead exposure, but the findings for these cognitive domains are less consistent across studies.

Lead and ADHD

In addition to the parallels between the behavioral domains affected in lead-exposed and ADHD children, an association between BLL and a diagnosis of ADHD has been reported in a number of recent studies (Table 2; see also Braun et al. 2006; Froehlich et al. 2009; Ha et al. 2009; Nigg et al. 2008, 2010; Roy et al. 2009; Wang et al. 2008). Importantly, this relationship was reported even at BLLs well below the CDC’s 10-μg/dL action level. No published studies were identified that looked for but did not observe an association between BLL and ADHD, although one study (Ha et al. 2009) reported an association that did not reach the traditional p = 0.05 threshold for statistical significance.
Table 2 Childhood lead exposure and ADHD.
Diagnostic measureCohort (n; age)OR (95% CI) or OutcomeaReference
Current stimulant medication
Parent report of ADHD diagnosed by a doctor
NHANES 1999–2002 (4,707; 4–15 years)4.3 (1.2–14.0)Braun et al. 2006
DSM-IV-TRNHANES 2001–2004 (2,588; 8–15 years)2.3 (1.5–3.8)Froehlich et al. 2009
Conners ADHD scaleKorea (1,778; school age)1.98 (0.76–5.13) (BLL > 3.5 vs. < 1.0 μg/dL)Ha et al. 2009
Independent diagnosis by two cliniciansCase–control (150; 8–17 years)↑BLL in ADHD, combined type compared with non-ADHD childrenNigg et al. 2008
Independent diagnosis by two cliniciansCase–control (236; 6–17 years)↑BLL in ADHD, combined type compared with non-ADHD childrenNigg et al. 2010
Conners ADHD scale; CADS; BRIEFIndia (756; 3–7 years)↑scores on the CADS ADHD indexRoy et al. 2009
DSM-IV-TRChina (case–control) (630 ADHD, 630 control; 4–12 years)6.0 (4.10–8.77) (BLL ≥ 10 vs. BLL ≤ 5 μg/dL)Wang et al. 2008
Abbreviations: BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; CADS, Conners ADHD/Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV scales; CI, confidence interval; DSM-IV-TR, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed., Text Revision; NHANES, National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey; OR, odds ratio.
a
↑indicates significant increase associated with lead exposure.
A study using the NHANES data (Braun et al. 2006) reported that the prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis or treatment was four times higher among children with BLL > 2.0 μg/dL compared with those with BLL < 0.8 μg/dL. Additional studies in Korea (Ha et al. 2009), China (Wang et al. 2008), India (Roy et al. 2009), and the United States (Froehlich et al. 2009) also suggest a link between BLL and a diagnosis of ADHD (Table 2).
Although evidence is beginning to emerge linking lead exposure to ADHD even at relatively low levels of exposure, the data are currently insufficient to infer causality. Furthermore, it is not clear whether behaviors exhibited by ADHD children could result in increased exposure to lead rather than (or in addition to) a direct contribution of lead exposure to the pathogenesis of ADHD. At this juncture, we are not aware of any studies that have attempted to address this difficult but important question.

Animal models

Numerous studies have used rodent and primate models to assess the effects of early lead exposure on tests of executive functions similar to those affected in ADHD children [Table 3; see also reviews by Cory-Slechta (2003) and Rice (1993, 1996, 2000)]. The findings reveal that early lead exposure impairs performance on tasks that assess spatial working memory, response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and temporal information processing. Attentional processes also seem to be affected, although relatively few animal studies have specifically assessed attention. Adverse effects have been reported at BLLs as low as 11–13 μg/dL (Rice 1993). As summarized above, this is similar to the BLLs associated with adverse effects in children. Descriptions of the various tests used in animal models are given in Supplemental Material, Table 2 (doi:10.1289/ehp.0901852).
Table 3 Animal studies of lead effects on cognitive domains affected in ADHD.
TestExposure periodBLL (μg/dL)aEffectbReference
Working memory

 Monkey studies
  DSAInfant → adult174 P, 68 SS↓% correct, ↑errorsLevin and Bowman 1986, Levin et al. 1987c
  DSAInfant → adult32–65 SS↓ errorsLevin and Bowman 1988
  DSAInfant → juvenile, infant → adult, or juvenile → adult19–36 P↑% incorrect all groupsRice and Gilbert 1990a
  DSAInfant → adult15–25 P, 11–13 SS↑% incorrectRice and Karpinski 1988
 Rat studies
  DSAJuvenile Md,e26–123 PNo effectMilar et al. 1981
  DSAJuvenile → adult M23–49 SS↑% correct, ↓ errorsCory-Slechta et al. 1991
  DSAJuvenile F20–36 SS↓% correctAlber and Strupp 1996

Response inhibition

 Monkey studies
  DRLInfant → adult115 P, 33 SS↑responding, ↓ IRTRice 1992
  DRLInfant → adult15–25 P, 11–13 SS↑respondingRice and Gilbert 1985
  FI, FR-FIfInfant → adult115 P, 33 SSInfant: ↓ pause timeg
Juvenile: ↑responding, ↓ IRT, ↑pause time
Rice 1988
  FI-EXThInfant → adult50–60 P, 20–30 SSFI: ↑responding, ↓ IRT
EXT: ↑responding
Rice et al. 1979
  FI, EXTInfant → adult32–65 SSFI: ↑IOC,i EXT: no effectMele et al. 1984
  FIInfant → adult15–25P, 11–13 SS↓ IRTRice 1985b
  DSAInfant → juvenile, infant → adult, or juvenile → adult19–36 P↑premature responses, ↑perseverative errors all groupsRice and Gilbert 1990a
DSA  Infant → adult15–25 P, 11–13 SS↑perseverative errorsRice and Karpinski 1988
 Rat studies
  DRL,   EXTJuvenile Mj59–186 PNo effectKishi et al. 1983
  DRLJuvenilej33–226 P, 15–56 SS↑respondingkOvermann 1977
  FIJuvenile → adult M14–54 SS↑respondingCory-Slechta et al. 1983
  FIJuvenile → adult M6–43 SSLow/moderate doses: ↑responding, ↓ pause time
High dose: ↓ responding, ↑pause time
Cory-Slechta and Thompson 1979
FI  Juvenile → adult Ml23 SS↓ respondingCory-Slechta 1990
  FIJuvenile → adult M13–23 SS↑respondingCory-Slechta and Pokora 1991
  FIJuvenile → adult M10–20 SS↑responding, ↓ IRTCory-Slechta et al. 1985
  FI(1) preconception → lactation or (2) preconception → adultNot reported(2) only: ↓ reinforcersZenick et al. 1979
  FR-waitJuvenile → adult M11–29 SS↑responding, ↓ waitingBrockel and Cory-Slechta 1998
  DSAJuvenile → adult M23–49 SS↓ premature responsesCory-Slechta et al. 1991
  Repeat acq-performmJuvenile → adult M25–74 SSAcquisition only: ↑premature responsesCohn et al. 1993
  Signal det w/distractnJuvenile → adolescent F40–140 P, 13–31 SS↑premature responses at higher Pb dosesStangle et al. 2007
  Signal det w/distractJuvenile → adult M16–28 SS↑commission errors at higher Pb doseBrockel and Cory-Slechta 1999

Cognitive flexibility

 Monkey studies
  Rev Lrn S/NSInfant → adult40–90 SS↑errors, ↑omissionsBushnell and Bowman 1979b
  Rev Lrn SInfant → juvenile32–65 SSNo effect on errorsBushnell and Bowman 1979a
  Rev Lrn S/NS(1) infant → juvenile, (2) infant → adult, or (3) juvenile → adult19–36 P• (1) ↑errors S, NS• (2) ↑errors S• (3) ↑errors S, NSRice 1990, Rice and Gilbert 1990b
  Rev Lrn SInfant → adult50–60 P, 20–30 SS↑errorsRice and Willes 1979
  Rev Lrn S/NSInfant → adult15–25 P, 11–13 SS↑errorsRice 1985a, Gilbert and Rice 1987
  RI-RIGestation21–70 SSo↓ transition rateNewland et al. 1994
 Rat studies
  Rev Lrn NSJuvenilej33–226 P, 15–56 SS↓% correctOvermann 1977
  Rev Lrn S/NSLactation or lactation → adult M36–57 P, 37–43 SSNo effectHastings et al. 1984
  Rev Lrn S/NSPreconception → adult F20–36 SS↑errorsHilson and Strupp 1997
  Rev Lrn NSGestation → lactation or lactation F131–158 P, 12–18 SSNo effect on errorsGaravan et al. 2000
  Repeat Acq-PerformJuvenile → adult M25–74 SS↓% correctCohn et al. 1993

Vigilance

 Rat studies
  Signal detpGestation → lactation or lactation F131–158 P, 12–18 SS↑omissionsMorgan et al. 2001
  Signal detJuvenile → adolescent F40–140 P, 13–31 SS↑omissions at higher Pb doseStangle et al. 2007
  Signal detJuvenile → adult M16–28 SS↑omission errors at lower Pb doseBrockel and Cory-Slechta 1999

Selective attention

 Rat studies
  Signal det w/distractJuvenile → adolescent F40–140 P, 13–31 SSNo effectStangle et al. 2007
Abbreviations: DRL, differential reinforcement of low rates of responding; DSA, delayed spatial alternation; EXT, extinction; FI, fixed interval; IOC, index of curvature; IRT, interresponse times; Rev Lrn S/NS, reversal learning spatial/nonspatial; RI-RI, random interval–random interval.
a
P indicates peak and SS indicates steady state BLL. Values for multiple treatment groups are expressed as a range. Control group values are not included.
b
↑indicates significant increase with lead treatment; ↓ indicates significant decrease.
c
DSA testing was repeated on the same monkeys 2 years after original testing; abnormalities persisted.
d
Male (M) or female (F) symbol indicates only that sex was tested.
e
Pups were gavaged from postnatal days 3–30 instead of exposure through nursing.
f
Monkeys were tested on an FI schedule as infants and a combined fixed ratio (FR)-FI schedule as juveniles. Exposure continued to adulthood.
g
Postreinforcement pause.
h
FI-EXT indicates that FI and EXT schedules were alternated within sessions. FI, EXT indicates that EXT schedule was implemented after FI testing.
i
Indicates an accelerated pattern of responding.
j
Pups were gavaged from postnatal days 3–21 instead of exposure through nursing.
k
Overmann (1977) began DRL testing at 67 days, whereas Kishi et al. (1983) began at ~150 days, by which time BLL would have fallen to a greater extent.
l
Differed from other FI studies from the same lab in that rats were dosed for longer periods (8 or 11 months) before FI testing started.
m
Multiple repeated acquisition-performance schedule.
n
Signal detection task with irrelevant (distracting) cues.
o
Range of maternal BLLs; offspring BLLs were not evaluated.
p
Signal detection task in which the cue to respond occurred at varied intervals.

Executive function

Working memory

Lead has been shown to impair performance on the delayed spatial alternation (DSA) task, a commonly used test of spatial working memory. Despite differences in experimental procedures, developmentally lead-exposed monkeys in several studies conducted in two different labs were impaired in their ability to learn the DSA task (Levin and Bowman 1986; Levin et al. 1987; Rice and Gilbert 1990a; Rice and Karpinski 1988). In both labs, lead-exposed monkeys were more likely to press the same lever repeatedly (perseverate) rather than alternate between levers. Similar findings were reported in rats exposed to lead as juveniles and tested on a DSA task (Alber and Strupp 1996), although the findings in rats are more inconsistent (Cory-Slechta et al. 1991).

Response inhibition

Although the findings are inconsistent in children, impairments of response inhibition after developmental lead exposure are well documented in animal models (Table 3). Early lead exposure increases rates of responding during both FI training and extinction in monkeys [reviewed by Rice (1993)] and during FI training in rats [reviewed by Cory-Slechta (2003)], similar to the findings seen in ADHD children (Sagvolden et al. 1998) and in an animal model of ADHD, the spontaneously hypertensive rat (Sagvolden 2000), when tested on FI tasks. The effects of lead exposure on response inhibition also were examined in lead-exposed monkeys using a DRL schedule in which early responses reset the clock, postponing reinforcement (Rice 1992; Rice and Gilbert 1985). Monkeys with moderate BLLs (11–13 μg/dL) learned the DRL task more slowly, but eventually achieved reinforcement rates indistinguishable from controls. However, monkeys with higher BLLs (33 μg/dL) had fewer reinforced responses and shorter average interresponse times.
FI and DRL schedules examine a different aspect of response inhibition than the stopping tasks often used in ADHD children. On FI and DRL schedules, the subject must withhold responding in order to achieve efficient performance, whereas on stopping tasks the subject must stop a response after it has already been initiated (Aguiar et al. 2010). This aspect of response inhibition has not been examined in studies of lead-exposed humans or animals.
Temporal information processing deficits, which are present in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010), also contribute to efficient performance on both DRL and FI schedules but may be hard to disentangle from response inhibition deficits. In another study that more directly evaluated the ability to estimate time, rats exposed to lead as juveniles were evaluated on a minimum response duration task (Cory-Slechta et al. 1981). Lead-exposed rats had shorter response durations and earned fewer reinforcers. These results in monkeys and rats suggest that, as with ADHD, lead may interfere with the ability to use internal cues to accurately predict time.

Cognitive flexibility

Discrimination reversal learning, which has parallels with the WCST, has been used to assess cognitive flexibility in lead-exposed animals. As discussed above, relatively low BLLs have been associated with impairments on the WCST in several human studies (Chiodo et al. 2004; Stiles and Bellinger 1993; Surkan et al. 2007). Impairments on the WCST are also observed in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010).
An early study found that juvenile rhesus monkeys with high BLLs showed impairments on spatial, color, and size reversal learning tasks, with the most striking deficits observed on the first reversal after original learning (Bushnell and Bowman 1979a, 1979b). Subsequently, reversal learning deficits were found in monkeys exposed to lower levels of lead (BLLs of 11–20 μg/dL) [reviewed by Rice (1993)]. Higher BLLs impaired both initial discrimination learning and reversals, whereas the effects of lower BLLs were evident on reversals but not on original learning. Response latencies did not differ between exposed and control monkeys, indicating the effects were not due to increased reaction time (Gilbert and Rice 1987). Reversal learning deficits were also reported in rats exposed to lead chronically from conception (Garavan et al. 2000; Hilson and Strupp 1997), although response pattern analyses suggested that lead-exposed rats exhibited an associative or learning deficit rather than a problem with cognitive flexibility.
Concurrent random interval–random interval (RI-RI) schedules, which allow pressing on two response levers to be reinforced at different frequencies, are another way to evaluate the ability to change response strategies. Lead-exposed monkeys were found to be impaired in their ability to transition between levers in an RI-RI task (Newland et al. 1994).

Attention

Relatively few animal studies have assessed attentional processes after lead exposure. Performance on vigilance tasks was impaired in rats postnatally exposed to lead (Brockel and Cory-Slechta 1999; Morgan et al. 2001; Stangle et al. 2007). Significant increases in omission errors, indicative of a deficit in vigilance, and premature responses, suggesting impaired inhibitory control, were observed. However, the same rats did not exhibit a selective attention deficit when a distracting stimulus was introduced (Stangle et al. 2007).

Neurochemistry

Lead-exposed animals also have underlying neurochemical deficits that share commonalities with ADHD, including reduced dopamine signaling (e.g., Cory-Slechta 1997; Levin et al. 1987). However, not all studies are concordant. Zuch et al. (1998) demonstrated that lead may increase dopaminergic signaling in mesolimbic pathways. Changes in dopamine signaling have been shown to play a role in the lead-induced deficits on both FI and DSA tasks, but not in deficits on a repeated acquisition task (Cory-Slechta 1997; Levin et al. 1987). For example, the prefrontal cortex—a brain region that has been implicated in ADHD (Aguiar et al. 2010)—is critical for accurate performance on the DSA task (e.g., Izaki et al. 2008; Sloan et al. 2006), with reductions in prefrontal dopamine producing impairments on short-delay DSA tasks in both rodents and nonhuman primates (Brozoski et al. 1979; Bubser and Schmidt 1990). In line with this, DSA deficits in lead-exposed animals were most pronounced at the shorter delays or were constant across delays (Alber and Strupp 1996; Levin and Bowman 1986). Also, treatment with the dopamine precursor l-dopa ameliorated the lead-induced deficit (Levin et al. 1987).
In summary, the cognitive domains impaired in lead-exposed animals parallel those affected in children exposed to lead (Canfield et al. 2004; Chiodo et al. 2004) and who have ADHD (Aguiar et al. 2010). There are also parallels between the neurochemical alterations underlying the lead-induced cognitive impairments and the neurochemical alterations believed to underlie ADHD (Brennan and Arnsten 2008).

Polychlorinated Biphenyls

Epidemiologic research

Researchers of both PCBs (Rice 2000) and ADHD (Nigg 2006) have pointed out that prenatal exposure to PCBs results in behavioral impairments that share significant commonalities with ADHD. In a recent review, Boucher et al. (2009) provide a synthesis of findings indicating that prenatal PCB exposure is associated with deficits on tasks that assess functions deficient in ADHD children. Table 4 presents the findings from epidemiologic studies on PCBs. It is difficult to compare PCB exposure across studies because of the different analytical techniques that have been used and the different subsets of PCB congeners that have been measured. Longnecker et al. (2003) addressed this by comparing PCB exposure across several studies using median concentrations of the most prevalent PCB congener (PCB-153). They found that the median PCB-153 concentrations in studies conducted in Michigan and the Netherlands ranged from 100 to 120 ng/g, whereas the median in the Oswego cohort was lower (40 ng/g). These studies are presented below.
Table 4 Prenatal PCB exposure and performance on tests of functions impaired in ADHD.
Domain/testCohortAge (years)OutcomeaReference
Working memory

 Verbal
  MSCAMichigan4.0↓ verbal and numerical memory scoresJacobson et al. 1990
 Netherlands6.5↓ memory scale scoresbVreugdenhil et al. 2002
  WISC-IIIOswego9.0↓ freedom from Distractibility scoresStewart et al. 2008
  WISC-RMichigan11.0↓ digit span scores (FF)Jacobson and Jacobson 2003
  SternbergMichigan4.0, 11.0↓ number correct (FF)Jacobson and Jacobson 2003
  AVLTNetherlands9.0— short delay recallVreugdenhil et al. 2004ac
 Nonverbal
  CorsidMichigan11.0— number correctJacobson and Jacobson 2003
  SeashoreeMichigan11.0↓ number correct (BF)Jacobson and Jacobson 2003

Response inhibition

 CPTOswego4.5, 8.0, 9.5↑commission errorsStewart et al. 2003, 2005
 Michigan4.0— commission errors (FF)Jacobson et al. 1992
 Michigan11.0↑commission errors (FF)Jacobson and Jacobson 2003
 SternbergfMichigan4.0↑commission errorsJacobson and Jacobson 2003
 DRLOswego9.5↓ interresponse timesStewart et al. 2006

Cognitive flexibility

 WCSTMichigan11.0↑perseverative errors (FF)
↓ categories completed (FF)
Jacobson and Jacobson 2003
 StroopgMichigan11.0↓ scores (allh)Jacobson and Jacobson 2003

Planning

 TOLNetherlands9.0↑no. trials to solveVreugdenhil et al. 2004ac
 ROCFNetherlands9.0— copy strategyVreugdenhil et al. 2004ac

Attention

 Vigilance
  CPTMichigan4.0, 11.0— omission errorsJacobson and Jacobson 2003
 Oswego4.5, 8.0, 9.5— response accuracyStewart et al. 2003, 2005
 Alertness
  CPTMichigan4.0, 11.0— reaction timeJacobson and Jacobson 2003
 Oswego4.5, 8.0, 9.5Reaction time not reportedStewart et al. 2003, 2005
  KVDMichigan4.0↑reaction timeJacobson et al. 1992
  SRTTNetherlands9.0↑reaction timeVreugdenhil et al. 2004ac
  Digit canceliMichigan11.0↑omission errors (FF)Jacobson and Jacobson 2003
  Mental rotationMichigan11.0↑reaction time (all)Jacobson and Jacobson 2003
Abbreviations: AVLT, Auditory-Visual Learning Test; BF, breast-fed; CPT, continuous performance task; DRL, differential reinforcement of low rates of responding; FF, formula-fed; KVD, Kagan Matching Familiar Figures Visual Discrimination task; MCSA, McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities; ROCF, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; SRTT, Simple Reaction Time test; TOL, Tower of London; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WISC-R, Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children–Revised; WISC-III, Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children III.
a
Only studies that assessed children on tests of specific functional domains relevant to ADHD are included (↑indicates significant increase associated with PCB exposure; ↓ indicates significant decrease; — indicates no association).
b
Association observed only in children whose mothers were younger and whose parents had lower IQ scores.
c
Assessed 42 children with low PCB exposure and 41 with high PCB exposure from a subset of 207 children from the original cohort.
d
The Corsi test is a visual-spatial analogue of the digit span test.
e
Seashore Rhythm test.
f
Sternberg Memory test.
g
Stroop Color-Word test.
h
Both formula-fed and breast-fed.
i
Digit cancellation.

PCBs and executive function

Working memory

Scores on working memory tests appear to be inversely associated with prenatal PCB exposure. In a prospective study, Vreugdenhil et al. (2002) evaluated the performance of 372 Dutch children 6.5 years of age (Netherlands cohort) on the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA) Memory Scale. Poorer scores were associated with PCB exposure, but only in formula-fed children of younger mothers and parents with lower verbal IQ scores (n = 178), suggesting that children from more disadvantaged backgrounds may be more at risk. In a separate prospective birth cohort from Oswego, Stewart et al. (2008) assessed verbal working memory in 156 of the original 293 children at 9 years of age using the WISC-III. Decreased freedom from distractibility scores, indicative of poorer verbal working memory, were associated with PCB exposure.
Similarly, in a prospective birth cohort from Michigan, deficits on working memory tasks were associated with prenatal PCB exposure at 4 years of age (n = 205–219) (Jacobson et al. 1990, 1992) and 11 years of age (n = 145–152) (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003). These associations included poorer memory scores on the MSCA at age 4, deficits in number correct on the Sternberg Memory test at both ages and on the WISC-R Digit Span at age 11, and poorer performance on the Seashore Rhythm test, an assessment of nonverbal auditory working memory, at age 11. There was no association between prenatal PCB exposure and scores on the Corsi Spatial Span task, a measure of working memory in the visual-spatial domain. With most tests an association was observed only when the analysis was limited to children who were not breast-fed (n = 44–52) (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003). Later the authors published evidence that mothers with higher IQs are both more likely to breast-feed and more likely to provide a better intellectual environment for their children, resulting in fewer adverse outcomes (Jacobson and Jacobson 2006). These findings and those from the Netherlands cohort suggest that other social or environmental factors can modify the effects of environmental exposures.
In conclusion, PCB exposure appears to affect performance on verbal working memory tests, but there has not been sufficient research to determine whether performance on nonverbal working memory tests is affected. By comparison, lead appears to affect performance on working memory tests to a lesser extent, whereas ADHD children are impaired on both verbal and nonverbal working memory tasks, with moderate effect sizes (Aguiar et al. 2010).

Response inhibition

Two groups have reported associations between prenatal PCB exposure and deficits in response inhibition. In the Oswego cohort, children with higher cord serum PCB levels made more errors of commission on CPTs at ages 4.5 years (n = 197) (Stewart et al. 2003), 8 years (n = 182) (Stewart et al. 2005), and 9.5 years (n = 183) (Stewart et al. 2005). The clearest evidence of a deficit was observed when the percentage of target stimuli was large relative to nontarget stimuli, indicating that when the children were required to respond more frequently to targets, they were less able to inhibit responding for nontargets. There was also an inverse association at 9.5 years of age between prenatal PCB exposure and interresponse times on a DRL task (Stewart et al. 2006). Children with higher PCB levels tended to respond too soon and earn fewer rewards. These significant findings from the DRL study are concurrent with, but separate from, the findings related to lead exposure in this cohort, as discussed above.
In the Michigan cohort, Jacobson et al. (1992) did not observe an increase in errors of commission on a similar CPT at 4 years of age but did see an increase in commission errors in a version of the Sternberg Memory task at 4 years and on a CPT at 11 years (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003). However, this relationship was only seen in the subset of children who were not breast-fed. ADHD children also make more commission errors on CPTs (Aguiar et al. 2010), and the DRL findings are similar to those of ADHD children performing on an FI schedule (Sagvolden et al. 1998). In contrast, lead exposure has not been associated with an increased frequency of commission errors in children performing CPTs with the exception of the German study (Walkowiak et al. 1998).

Cognitive flexibility and planning

Cognitive flexibility and planning have not been well studied in PCB-exposed children. The WCST and the Stroop Color-Word test, both of which assess cognitive flexibility, have been used only in the Michigan study (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003). Children with greater prenatal PCB exposure had more difficulty changing response strategy when the stimulus dimension changed on the WCST, but the finding was significant only in the subset of children that were formula-fed. No significant findings were seen on the Stroop test. These results are similar to those observed in lead-exposed children. ADHD children are impaired on both the WCST and Stroop tests, although the findings on the Stroop test are less consistent (Aguiar et al. 2010). Vreugdenhil et al. (2004a) evaluated 9-year-olds from a subgroup of the Netherlands cohort (n = 207) who had the lowest (n = 42) and highest (n = 41) prenatal PCB exposure on tests of planning ability. Deficits were observed on the TOL task but not on the ROCF in the children with higher exposure. Performance on these and other planning tasks is impaired in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010) and, in some studies, in lead-exposed children (Table 1).

PCBs and attention

Interestingly, no significant associations have been observed between prenatal PCB exposure and omission errors (a measure of vigilance) on CPTs. This was true for children at different ages: 4.5, 8, and 9.5 years in the Oswego cohort (Stewart et al. 2003, 2005) and 4 and 11 years in the Michigan cohort (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003). This differs from the findings of increased omission errors on CPTs in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010) and in some lead studies (Chiodo et al. 2004, 2007; Walkowiak et al. 1998). In contrast, prenatal exposure to PCBs has been associated with slower reaction times (a measure of alertness) in both the Michigan cohort (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003; Jacobson et al. 1992) and the Netherlands cohort (Vreugdenhil et al. 2004a, 2004b).
In the Michigan cohort, higher prenatal PCB exposure was associated with slower reaction times in a visual discrimination task at 4 years (Jacobson et al. 1992) and in a mental rotation task at 11 years (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003). Additionally, PCB exposure was associated with more errors of omission on a digit cancellation task, which also suggests an impaired ability to stay alert. In contrast, CPT reaction times were not affected at either 4 or 11 years (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003). The differential effects on reaction time across tasks could reflect different information processing requirements of the tasks.
Vreugdenhil et al. (2004a) compared reaction times on a modified version of Letz’s (1994) Simple Reaction Time test in the two groups of 9-year-olds from the Netherlands cohort who had the lowest and highest prenatal PCB exposure. Low-exposure children were significantly faster than high-exposure children. Vreugdenhil et al. (2004b) also measured the magnitude of the P300 event-related potential, which is a neurophysiologic measure of attentional processing time, in the same sample of 9-year-old children during an auditory task. High-exposure children had significantly longer P300 latencies, again suggesting an association between prenatal PCB exposure and slower processing speed. Although these findings are potentially important, they should be interpreted with caution given that findings observed in a subgroup of the original population (i.e., Jacobson and Jacobson 2003; Vreugdenhil et al. 2004a, 2004b) can be difficult to replicate in epidemiologic research.
In summary, PCB-exposed children show evidence of impaired verbal working memory, response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and alertness on various tasks, but vigilance, as assessed on CPTs, appears to be relatively intact. In comparison, lead-exposed children demonstrate impaired cognitive flexibility, vigilance, and alertness, as well as less consistent deficits in working memory and planning, but only limited effects on response inhibition. Deficits in all of these functions are typical in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010).

Animal models

A variety of studies have used animal models to assess the effects of PCB exposure on executive functions impaired in ADHD children (Table 5). Many of the same tests used to assess the effects of lead exposure on executive function have been employed, allowing for comparisons across the two contaminants.
Table 5 Animal studies of PCB effects on cognitive domains affected in ADHD.
TestExposure periodPCBsaEffectbReference
Working memory

 Monkey studies
  DSAPreconception → infantAroclor 1016No effectLevin et al. 1988
  DSAPreconceptioncAroclor 1248↓% correct, ↑errorsLevin et al. 1988
  DSAInfant MdMilk mixturee↑errorsRice and Hayward 1997
 Rat studies
  DSAGestationPCB-28, -118, or -153F only: ↓ no. correct, ↓ acquisitionSchantz et al. 1995
  DSAGestationPCB-95No effectSchantz et al. 1997
  DSAGestation or gestation → lactationfAroclor 1016No effectgZahalka et al. 2001
  DSAGestation → lactationAroclor 1254No effectgZahalka et al. 2001
  DSAPreconception → lactationAroclor 1254↓% correct, ↑errorsWidholm et al. 2004

Response inhibition

 Monkey studies
  DRLInfantMilk mixture↑responding,h ↓ IRTRice 1998
  FIPreconception → lactationAroclor 1248No effectMele et al. 1986
  FI w/reinf omissionsiPreconceptionjAroclor 1248↑respondingkMele et al. 1986
  FI w/reinf omissionsPreconception → lactationlAroclor 1248No effectMele et al. 1986
  FR-FIInfant MMilk mixture↓ IRT, ↓ pause timemRice 1997
  DSAInfant MMilk mixture↑perseverative errorsRice and Hayward 1997
 Rat studies
  DRL, EXTnPreconception → lactationFox River mixoDRL: no effect, EXT: ↑responding, ↓ IRTSable et al. 2006
  DRLPreconception → lactationFox River mix↓ reinforced: nonreinforced responsesSable et al. 2009
  VI-DRLLactation FPCB-153↓ IRTHolene et al. 1999
  FI-EXTpLactation MPCB-153FI: ↑responding, ↑perseverative pressing, EXT: ↑respondingHolene et al. 1998
  FI-EXTLactation FPCB-153No effectHolene et al. 1999
  FIPreconception → adult MClophen A30↑responding in highest dose groupLilienthal et al. 1990
  FI-EXTAdolescent → adult MAroclor 1248FI & EXT: ↑respondingBerger et al. 2001
  FI w/reinf omissionsGestation → lactationAroclor 1254F only: ↑respondingkTaylor et al. 2002

Cognitive flexibility

 Monkey studies
  Rev Lrn S/NSPreconception → lactationAroclor 1016S: ↓ acquisition, NS: ↑acquisitionqSchantz et al. 1989
  Rev Lrn S/NSPreconceptionrAroclor 1248No effectsSchantz et al. 1989
  Rev Lrn S/NSPreconceptiontAroclor 1248NS: ↑acquisitionqSchantz et al. 1989
  Rev Lrn S/NSPreconception → lactationAroclor 1248No effectsSchantz et al. 1989
  Rev Lrn S/NSPreconception → lactationuAroclor 1248S, NS: ↑errorsBowman et al. 1978
  Rev Lrn SInfant MMilk mixtureNo effectRice 1998
  Rev Lrn NSInfant MMilk mixture↑variability in response latenciesRice and Hayward 1997
  RI-RIInfant MMilk mixtureNo effectRice and Hayward 1999
 Rat studies
  Rev Lrn SGestation → lactationAroclor 1254↑errorsvWidholm et al. 2001

Vigilance

 Rat studies
  Signal detwGestation → lactationAroclor 1254No effectBushnell et al. 2002
Abbreviations: DRL, differential reinforcement of low rates of responding; EXT, extinction; FI, fixed interval; FR-FI, fixed ratio–fixed interval schedule; IRT, interresponse time; Rev Lrn S/NS, reversal learning spatial/nonspatial; RI-RI, random interval–random interval; VI-DRL, variable-interval DRL schedule.
a
PCB congeners or mixtures used in each study.
b
↑indicates significant increase with PCB treatment; ↓ indicates significant decrease.
c
Two different cohorts of monkeys were tested. Cohort 1 dams’ exposure ended 1 year before conception. The same dams were rebred 32 months after exposure for cohort 2.
d
Male (M) or female (F) symbol indicates only that sex was tested.
e
PCB mixture representative of congeners in human breast milk.
f
One group of pups was exposed through nursing, whereas a second group was exposed through nursing and gavaged daily from postnatal day 3–21.
g
DSA testing occurred in five sessions over postnatal days 22–23. Rats were younger and were tested for a shorter period than in other DSA studies.
h
Only nonreinforced responses were increased.
i
FI with reinforcement omissions; on a percentage of correct trials, reinforcers were omitted (w/reinf).
j
Offspring of the same dams in the previous FI experiment (Mele et al. 1986). Dams had not received PCBs for 20 months at the time of the second breeding.
k
Increased responding was only seen after trials in which reinforcers were omitted.
l
Dams received PCB doses that were 20% of those used in the previous study (Mele et al. 1986).
m
Postreinforcement pause.
n
Extinction schedule implemented after DRL testing.
o
PCB mixture representative of congeners in sport-caught fish.
p
FI-EXT indicates FI and EXT schedules were alternated within sessions.
q
Facilitated acquisition of a shape problem occurred after shape was used as an irrelevant cue in a prior problem. This may represent failure of the PCB-exposed monkeys to learn the irrelevancy of shape in the prior problem.
r
The same first cohort of monkeys tested on the DSA task (Levin et al. 1988).
s
Irrelevant cues were not employed in this group of monkeys.
t
The same second cohort of monkeys tested on the DSA task (Levin et al. 1988).
u
Dams in this experiment received PCB doses that were 2.5 times greater than those used in Schantz et al. (1989).
v
Male rats had ↑errors on first reversal due to ↑perseverative responses. Females had ↑errors on fourth and fifth reversals, suggesting impaired ability to make new associations.
w
Signal detection task in which the cue to respond occurred at varied intervals.

Executive function

Working memory

Performance on the DSA working memory task is impaired by early PCB exposure in a number of studies [Levin et al. 1988; Rice and Hayward 1997; Schantz et al. 1995; Widholm et al. 2004; reviewed by Sable and Schantz (2006)], although some studies did not report any effects (Levin et al. 1988; Schantz et al. 1997; Zahalka et al. 2001). Rice and Hayward (1997) found that monkeys treated with an experimental mixture of PCBs in the early postnatal period showed impaired acquisition of the task, responding repeatedly at the same location (perseverating) rather than alternating responses between the two locations. The monkeys also made more errors at short delays. Similar findings were reported in monkeys exposed to the commercial PCB mixture Aroclor 1248 (Levin et al. 1988), in female but not in male rats exposed to individual PCB congeners (Schantz et al. 1995), and in rats of both sexes exposed to Aroclor 1254 (Widholm et al. 2004). The findings of perseverative lever pressing on the DSA task parallel those observed with lead exposure in monkeys and rodents.

Response inhibition

Performance on tests of response inhibition including FI and DRL operant schedules is also impaired by early PCB exposure. Rice (1997, 1998) found that the same treated monkeys that were impaired on the DSA task made more responses with shorter interresponse times on both FI and DRL schedules. As described above, excessive responding with shorter interresponse times on a DRL schedule was correlated with both PCB and lead exposure in children from the Oswego cohort (Stewart et al. 2006).
The findings of impaired response inhibition in monkeys have been mirrored in several rodent studies. Lactational exposure of rats to PCB-153 produced increased responding during FI training and extinction in male rats (Holene et al. 1998) but not in female rats (Holene et al. 1999). In a separate study, juvenile male rats exposed to either PCB-contaminated fish or Aroclor 1248 concentrations equivalent to those in the fish (Berger et al. 2001) showed increased lever pressing and bursts of responding on an FI task. Similar findings have been observed in ADHD children tested on FI schedules (Sagvolden et al. 1998). Perinatal exposure of rats to an environmental PCB mixture (Kostyniak et al. 2005) resulted in a decrease in the ratio of reinforced:nonreinforced lever presses on a DRL schedule (Sable et al. 2009), as well as higher rates of responding during an extinction schedule after DRL testing (Sable et al. 2006). The findings of impaired performance on FI and DRL schedules in PCB-exposed animals parallels the findings seen with lead-exposed animals tested on similar schedules.

Cognitive flexibility

Discrimination reversal learning tasks have been used to assess the effects of PCBs on cognitive flexibility in monkeys (Bowman et al. 1978; Rice 1998; Rice and Hayward 1997; Schantz et al. 1989) and in rats (Widholm et al. 2001). When deficits were observed, animals tended to be most impaired on the first reversal after original learning (Schantz et al. 1989; Widholm et al. 2001), with error pattern analyses suggesting this was related to increased perseverative responding (Widholm et al. 2001), similar to the increase in perseverative responding on the WCST observed in children with PCB exposure (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003), lead exposure (Chiodo et al. 2004; Stiles and Bellinger 1993; Surkan et al. 2007), and ADHD (Aguiar et al. 2010). Rice (2000) argued that impaired reversal learning seen with lead and, to a lesser extent, PCB exposure can be explained as an inability to alter established response strategies as opposed to an inability to reallocate mental resources in response to changing situational demands. As discussed above, an RI-RI schedule is another way to evaluate the ability to change response strategies. In contrast to lead-exposed monkeys (Newland et al. 1994), PCB-exposed monkeys were not impaired in their ability to transition between levers on an RI-RI task (Rice and Hayward 1999). The findings from both studies are consistent with Rice’s hypothesis if lead impairs the ability to detect the need for changing response strategies to a greater extent than PCBs.

Attention

Only one study has scrutinized the effects of PCBs on attentional processes. Bushnell et al. (2002) reported that perinatal exposure of rats to Aroclor 1254 did not affect performance on a signal detection task. Additional studies using the five-choice serial reaction time task to assess attention (Robbins 2002) are ongoing in our laboratory.

Neurochemistry

Neurochemical studies demonstrate that PCBs cause reductions in dopamine in the prefrontal cortex and striatum (Seegal et al. 1991, 1997). PCBs reduce functioning (Fonnum et al. 2006) and decrease expression (Caudle et al. 2006) of both the dopamine transporter and the vesicular monoamine transporter 2 in dopaminergic neurons in vitro, resulting in altered synaptic and cytosolic dopamine clearance. Thus, dysregulation of synaptic catecholamines could mediate some of the cognitive impairments observed after PCB exposure. Of interest, Sable et al. (2009) reported that amphetamine disrupted DRL performance less in PCB-exposed rats, providing a behavioral measure of altered brain dopamine function.
In summary, the findings from studies in animal models reveal deficits on tasks that assess many of the same cognitive domains affected by PCB exposure in humans including tests of working memory, response inhibition, and cognitive flexibility. Performance on these same tasks has been reported to be impaired in laboratory animals and children exposed to lead during early development and in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010). However, despite the obvious parallels between the cognitive and neurochemical alterations produced by PCBs and the cognitive and neurochemical impairments observed in ADHD children, the relationship of PCB exposure to a diagnosis of ADHD remains essentially unexplored. One exception is a recent study that reported a positive association between prenatal PCB exposure and ADHD-like behaviors as assessed on the Conners Rating Scale for Teachers (Sagiv et al. 2010).

Other Environmental Contaminants

Lead was phased out of gasoline and PCBs were phased out of use in electrical equipment in the 1970s. Since that time, environmental concentrations and human body burdens of both “legacy” contaminants have been slowly decreasing. In contrast, human exposure to other chemicals, including brominated flame retardants (Sjodin et al. 2008), bisphenol A (BPA) (Calafat et al. 2008), phthalates (Hauser and Calafat 2005), certain pesticides (Barr et al. 2005), and polyfluoroalkylated chemicals (PFCs) (Jensen and Leffers 2008), has become ubiquitous. The studies reviewed herein demonstrate a risk to children’s neurobehavioral development from lead and PCB exposure. Whether the risks of altered neurodevelopment from PCB exposure will continue into the future is unknown. However, childhood lead exposure is likely to remain an important public health issue because of the lead contamination in older housing in the United States and the significantly higher contamination that still exists in developing countries (Meyer et al. 2008). Although additional research to more fully delineate the role of lead and PCB exposure in the etiology of ADHD would be valuable, it is equally if not more important to gain a better understanding of potential neurodevelopmental effects of other “emerging” contaminants, of which relatively little is known.
Chemicals such as polybrominated diphenyl ethers (PBDEs) and BPA, which have been shown to disrupt dopamine signaling in vitro (Jones and Miller 2008; Mariussen and Fonnum 2003), should be investigated as possible contributing factors in ADHD. Although the effects of PBDE exposures in animal studies are observed at higher levels than those to which humans are exposed, some BPA studies have demonstrated adverse effects at levels comparable to those humans encounter daily (Richter et al. 2007; Vandenberg et al. 2010). Studies of attention or executive function after PBDE exposure are limited, but Driscoll et al. (2009) found that mice postnatally exposed to the commercial PBDE mixture DE-71 exhibited impulsivity and inattention, as evidenced by more premature responses and omission errors, respectively, on the five-choice serial reaction time task. Prenatal PBDE exposure also has been associated with impaired vigilance in a study of 62 children 5–6 years of age (Roze et al. 2009). PBDEs have been shown to reduce vesicular and, to a lesser extent, synaptosomal dopamine uptake in vitro (Mariussen and Fonnum 2003), paralleling what has been reported with PCBs (Caudle et al. 2006; Fonnum et al. 2006) and further supporting the idea that PBDE exposure could potentially be a risk factor for ADHD. BPA, a component in polycarbonate plastics, has been shown to reduce dopamine synthesis, release, and turnover, as well as dopamine transporter expression, in rodents [reviewed by Jones and Miller (2008)], but only a few studies have evaluated the effects of developmental exposure on cognitive function and none to date has employed tests relevant to ADHD.
A recently published study reported that prenatal exposure to low-molecular-weight phthalates was associated with poorer parent-rated scores on attention and externalizing problems (including impulsivity and hyperactivity) in 188 children 4–9 years of age using the Behavior Assessment System for Children scales (Engel et al. 2010). Preceding this, Kim et al. (2009) reported that higher urine phthalate metabolites at the time of testing in 261 children 8–11 years of age were associated with higher teacher-rated scores on both the inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity subscales of the ADHD Rating Scale, whereas CPT testing revealed more omission and commission errors, suggestive of impaired vigilance and response inhibition, but no increases in reaction time, suggesting that alertness was not affected.
A study of an inner-city minority population (n = 288) reported an association between prenatal chlorpyrifos exposure and scores in the clinical problems range on both the Attention Problems scale and ADHD scale of the Child Behavior Checklist at 3 years of age (Rauh et al. 2006). Although a study of 356 organophosphate-exposed children of farmworkers did not find a significant association with scores on either of these scales at 2 years of age (Eskenazi et al. 2007), the children were followed up at 3.5 years (n = 331) and 5 years (n = 323), with several significant associations observed at 5 years of age (Marks et al. 2010). Specifically, prenatal organophosphate exposure (as measured by maternal urinary dialkyl metabolites during pregnancy) was associated with increased scores on the Attention Problems scale and ADHD scale of the Child Behavior Checklist and with an increased ADHD confidence index on the Conners Kiddie CPT task. Finally, a cross-sectional study using NHANES data found that parentally reported ADHD was positively associated with urinary organophosphate metabolite levels (Bouchard et al. 2010). Only a few animal studies have addressed effects of pesticides on behavioral end points relevant to ADHD. Adult exposure of rats to chlorpyrifos impaired vigilance on a visual signal detection task (Bushnell et al. 2001; Samsam et al. 2005), whereas acute juvenile exposure transiently impaired performance on a DSA working memory task (Stanton et al. 1994).
Very recently, a positive association between parentally reported ADHD and serum PFC levels was reported based on an analysis of NHANES data (Hoffman et al. 2010). To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine a potential association between childhood PFC exposure and ADHD. A few animal studies have assessed neurobehavioral outcomes after PFC exposure, but we are not aware of any that have directly assessed cognitive functions relevant to ADHD.
Although in this review we focus on environmental contaminants, a number of other potential environmental factors, including premature birth, low birth weight, and psychosocial adversity, are known or suspected to be risk factors in the development of ADHD on their own and could potentially interact with environmental exposures to modify risk (Banerjee et al. 2007; Swanson et al. 2007). Converging evidence from a number of clinical and population-based studies also links both maternal smoking and maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy with ADHD (Braun et al. 2006; Pennington et al. 2009), with Froehlich et al. (2009) demonstrating that children with combined lead and prenatal tobacco exposure have a greater risk of ADHD than would be predicted if the individual risks were multiplied. Whether other risk factors interact with chemical exposures to increase ADHD risk is an area that should be further explored.

Conclusions and Future Directions

With the addition of studies conducted in the last decade, the specific profiles of cognitive deficits associated with exposure to lead and PCBs are coming into sharper focus. The deficits associated with both contaminants bear many similarities to the behavioral problems observed in ADHD children. Table 6 presents a comparison of cognitive domains affected in ADHD, lead, and PCBs. In the case of ADHD, confidence in the findings in Table 6 is based on the number of individual studies, the concordance of the findings, and the effect size of meta-analytic studies reviewed by Aguiar et al. (2010). For lead and PCBs, confidence in the findings is based on the number of individual studies and the concordance of findings across studies.
Table 6 Comparison of cognitive domains affected in ADHD and by lead and PCBs in humans and laboratory species: degree of confidence in findings.
  LeadPCBs
DomainADHDHumanAnimalHumanAnimal
Working memory
 Verbal+++++ +++ 
 Nonverbal (incl. spatial)++++++++++
Response inhibition++++++++++++++
Cognitive flexibility++++++++++++
Planning++++ + 
Attention
 Vigilance (sustained)++++++++
 Alertness+++++ ++ 
Pluses indicate range of degree of confidence, from low (+) to high (++++). — indicates that literature does not support involvement of the domain.
Both lead and PCBs have effects on tests designed to assess working memory, with deficits observed in humans (Boucher et al. 2009; Canfield et al. 2004; Chiodo et al. 2004; Surkan et al. 2007) and animal models (Alber and Strupp 1996; Rice 1993; Sable and Schantz 2006), although performance on working memory tests has not been consistently associated with lead in human studies (Table 1). Also, it is not clear that the deficits observed in animals on working memory tasks are related to actual impairments in working memory. Typically working memory performance will worsen as the delay interval between trials gets longer. However, the deficits seen with lead and PCBs were present even at the shortest delays and did not get more severe as the delay interval increased, suggesting that some other cognitive or behavioral impairment may underlie the deficit.
Interestingly, ADHD children show more pronounced deficits on tests that assess spatial working memory than on those that assess verbal working memory (Aguiar et al. 2010). Few epidemiologic studies of lead or PCBs have employed spatial working memory tasks. An exception is the study by Canfield et al. (2004), in which increased errors on the spatial span and spatial working memory tasks from the CANTAB battery, suggestive of spatial working memory deficits, were found to be associated with higher BLLs. Use of spatial working memory tests could provide additional information about the extent to which the functional domains affected with lead or PCB exposure parallel those seen in ADHD children.
Although uncertainty remains, studies in children and animal models suggest that lead may impair both response inhibition and attentional processes (Chiodo et al. 2004, 2007; Morgan et al. 2001; Stewart et al. 2006), whereas PCBs appear to impair response inhibition to a greater extent than attention (Bushnell et al. 2002; Jacobson and Jacobson 2003; Stewart et al. 2003, 2005, 2006). This distinction is most evident on CPTs, where lead-exposed children tend to exhibit an increase in omission errors (impaired vigilance) with less consistent increases in commission errors (impaired response inhibition), whereas PCB-exposed children show impairments in commission errors and no increase in omission errors or accuracy (Tables 1 and 4). The evidence from animal models also strongly supports that both lead [reviewed by Rice (1996)] and PCBs [reviewed by Sable and Schantz (2006)] impair response inhibition.
In addition to deficits in response inhibition and vigilance on CPTs, ADHD children are also impaired on the stop signal task (Aguiar et al. 2010). Commission errors on CPTs assess the ability to inhibit the initiation of a response, whereas the stop signal task evaluates the ability to stop a response once it has already been initiated (e.g., Walshaw et al. 2010). To our knowledge, this aspect of response inhibition has not been assessed in lead- or PCB-exposed children or animals. Use of this task could provide additional information about the extent to which the deficits in response inhibition after lead or PCB exposure parallel those seen in ADHD children.
Both contaminants also cause impairments on tasks that involve components of both response inhibition and temporal information processing, such as FI and DRL schedules. These findings are consistent across species, from rats to monkeys [see reviews by Cory-Slechta (2003); Rice (1993, 2000); Sable and Schantz (2006)] to humans (Stewart et al. 2006). Performance on temporal processing tasks is also impaired in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010).
Another interesting pattern that emerges when recent human studies are considered together with older animal research is a relatively consistent association between lead exposure and deficits in cognitive flexibility using such tasks as WCST in humans (Chiodo et al. 2004; Stiles and Bellinger 1993; Surkan et al. 2007) and reversal learning in monkeys [reviewed by Rice (1993)] and rats (Garavan et al. 2000; Hilson and Strupp 1997). Cognitive flexibility is also impaired in ADHD children (Aguiar et al. 2010). The one study that assessed cognitive flexibility after PCB exposure found deficits on the WCST in PCB-exposed children (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003). Deficits on reversal learning tasks are also present in PCB-exposed monkeys and rats [reviewed by Sable and Schantz (2006)]. Although the effects of PCBs are somewhat less consistent than for lead, this could relate to differences in the doses and timing of exposure across studies.
To date, most ADHD research has focused on the combined phenotype or has not differentiated among the three phenotypes (Aguiar et al. 2010). Another avenue for future research would be to investigate whether exposures to lead, PCBs, or other chemicals are differentially associated with specific ADHD phenotypes. Exposure to environmental contaminants may contribute to the heterogeneity in the expression of ADHD. An equally important objective will be to investigate whether there are specific gene polymorphisms, epigenetic changes, or other ADHD risk factors (low birth weight, preterm birth, psychosocial stress, male sex) that interact with PCBs, lead, or other chemicals to increase ADHD risk.
Finally, there is a relatively extensive body of literature reporting the effects of neurodevelopmental exposure to lead and PCBs in animals and humans. It is hoped that the knowledge gained from this review of the literature on these two legacy contaminants will assist researchers attempting to understand the potential contribution of emerging contaminants, including PBDEs, BPA, phthalates, pesticides and PFCs, to ADHD risk.

Article Notes

Supplemental Material is available online (doi:10.1289/ehp.0901852 via http://dx.doi.org/).
This work was funded in part by National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (NIEHS) grant ES015687 and Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry (ATSDR) grant TS000072 to S.L.S. and by NIEHS grant K08 ES017045 to P.A.E.
The contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NIEHS or ATSDR.

References

Aguiar A, Eubig PA, Schantz SL. 2010. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a focused overview for children’s environmental health researchers. Environ Health Perspect 118:1646-1653 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20829148/.
Alber SA, Strupp BJ. 1996. An in-depth analysis of lead effects in a delayed spatial alternation task: assessment of mnemonic effects, side bias, and proactive interference. Neurotoxicol Teratol 18((1)):3-15 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8700040/.
American Psychiatric Association. 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th ed. Text Revision Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association.
Baddeley A. 1986. Working Memory New York Oxford University Press.
Banerjee TD, Middleton F, Faraone SV. 2007. Environmental risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Paediatr 96((9)):1269-1274 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17718779/.
Barr DB, Allen R, Olsson AO, Bravo R, Caltabiano LM, Montesano Aet al. 2005. Concentrations of selective metabolites of organophosphorus pesticides in the United States population. Environ Res 99((3)):314-326 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16307973/.
Bellinger DC. 2008. Very low lead exposures and children’s neurodevelopment. Curr Opin Pediatr 20((2)):172-177 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18332714/.
Berger DF, Lombardo JP, Jeffers PM, Hunt AE, Bush B, Casey Aet al. 2001. Hyperactivity and impulsiveness in rats fed diets supplemented with either Aroclor 1248 or PCB-contaminated St. Lawrence river fish. Behav Brain Res 126((1–2)):1-11 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11704246/.
Bouchard MF, Bellinger DC, Wright RO, Weisskopf MG. 2010. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and urinary metabolites of organophosphate pesticides. Pediatrics 125((6)):e1270-e1277 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20478945/.
Boucher O, Muckle G, Bastien CH. 2009. Prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls: a neuropsychologic analysis. Environ Health Perspect 117:7-16 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19165381/.
Bowman RE, Heironimus MP, Allen JR. 1978. Correlation of PCB body burden with behavioral toxicology in monkeys. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 9((1)):49-56 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/100792/.
Braun JM, Kahn RS, Froehlich T, Auinger P, Lanphear BP. 2006. Exposures to environmental toxicants and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in U.S. children. Environ Health Perspect 114:1904-1909 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17185283/.
Brennan AR, Arnsten AF. 2008. Neuronal mechanisms underlying attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the influence of arousal on prefrontal cortical function. Ann NY Acad Sci 1129:236-245 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18591484/.
Brockel BJ, Cory-Slechta DA. 1998. Lead, attention, and impulsive behavior: changes in a fixed-ratio waiting-for-reward paradigm. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 60((2)):545-552 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9632239/.
Brockel BJ, Cory-Slechta DA. 1999. The effects of postweaning low-level Pb exposure on sustained attention: a study of target densities, stimulus presentation rate, and stimulus predictability. Neurotoxicology 20((6)):921-933 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10693973/.
Brozoski TJ, Brown RM, Rosvold HE, Goldman PS. 1979. Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. Science 205((4409)):929-932 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/112679/.
Bubser M, Schmidt WJ. 1990. 6-Hydroxydopamine lesion of the rat prefrontal cortex increases locomotor activity, impairs acquisition of delayed alternation tasks, but does not affect uninterrupted tasks in the radial maze. Behav Brain Res 37((2)):157-168 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2108704/.
Bushnell PJ, Bowman RE. 1979a. Persistence of impaired reversal learning in young monkeys exposed to low levels of dietary lead. J Toxicol Environ Health 5((6)):1015-1023 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/119054/.
Bushnell PJ, Bowman RE. 1979b. Reversal learning deficits in young monkeys exposed to lead. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 10((5)):733-742 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/115012/.
Bushnell PJ, Moser VC, MacPhail RC, Oshiro WM, Derr-Yellin EC, Phillips PMet al. 2002. Neurobehavioral assessments of rats perinatally exposed to a commercial mixture of polychlorinated biphenyls. Toxicol Sci 68((1)):109-120 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12075116/.
Bushnell PJ, Moser VC, Samsam TE. 2001. Comparing cognitive and screening tests for neurotoxicity. Effects of acute chlorpyrifos on visual signal detection and a neurobehavioral test battery in rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol 23((1)):33-44 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11274874/.
Calafat AM, Ye X, Wong LY, Reidy JA, Needham LL. 2008. Exposure of the U.S. population to bisphenol A and 4-tertiary-octylphenol: 2003–2004. Environ Health Perspect 116:39-44 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18197297/.
Canfield RL, Gendle MH, Cory-Slechta DA. 2004. Impaired neuropsychological functioning in lead-exposed children. Dev Neuropsychol 26((1)):513-540 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15276907/.
Canfield RL, Kreher DA, Cornwell C, Henderson CR. 2003. Low-level lead exposure, executive functioning, and learning in early childhood. Child Neuropsychol 9((1)):35-53 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12815521/.
Caudle WM, Richardson JR, Delea KC, Guillot TS, Wang M, Pennell KDet al. 2006. Polychlorinated biphenyl-induced reduction of dopamine transporter expression as a precursor to Parkinson’s disease-associated dopamine toxicity. Toxicol Sci 92((2)):490-499 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16702228/.
CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). 2005. Preventing Lead Poisoning in Young Children Atlanta, GA CDC Available: http://www.cdc.gov/nceh/lead/publications/PrevLeadPoisoning.pdf [accessed 20 August 2010].
Chiodo LM, Covington C, Sokol RJ, Hannigan JH, Jannise J, Ager Jet al. 2007. Blood lead levels and specific attention effects in young children. Neurotoxicol Teratol 29((5)):538-546 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17553667/.
Chiodo LM, Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL. 2004. Neurodevelopmental effects of postnatal lead exposure at very low levels. Neurotoxicol Teratol 26((3)):359-371 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15113598/.
Cohn J, Cox C, Cory-Slechta DA. 1993. The effects of lead exposure on learning in a multiple repeated acquisition and performance schedule. Neurotoxicology 14((2–3)):329-346 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8247407/.
Cory-Slechta DA. 1990. Exposure duration modifies the effects of low level lead of fixed-interval performance. Neurotoxicology 11((3)):427-441 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2284049/.
Cory-Slechta DA. 1997. Relationships between Pb-induced changes in neurotransmitter system function and behavioral toxicity. Neurotoxicology 18((3)):673-688 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9339816/.
Cory-Slechta DA. 2003. Lead-induced impairments in complex cognitive function: offerings from experimental studies. Child Neuropsychol 9((1)):54-75 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12815522/.
Cory-Slechta DA, Bissen ST, Young AM, Thompson T. 1981. Chronic postweaning lead exposure and response duration performance. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 60((1)):78-84 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/7281178/.
Cory-Slechta DA, Pokora MJ. 1991. Behavioral manifestations of prolonged lead exposure initiated at different stages of the life cycle: I. Schedule-controlled responding. Neurotoxicology 12((4)):745-760 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/1795899/.
Cory-Slechta DA, Pokora MJ, Widzowski DV. 1991. Behavioral manifestations of prolonged lead exposure initiated at different stages of the life cycle: II. Delayed spatial alternation. Neurotoxicology 12((4)):761-776 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/1795900/.
Cory-Slechta DA, Thompson T. 1979. Behavioral toxicity of chronic postweaning lead exposure in the rat. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 47((1)):151-159 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/425114/.
Cory-Slechta DA, Weiss B, Cox C. 1983. Delayed behavioral toxicity of lead with increasing exposure concentration. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 71((3)):342-352 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/6658785/.
Cory-Slechta DA, Weiss B, Cox C. 1985. Performance and exposure indices of rats exposed to low concentrations of lead. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 78((2)):291-299 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/4035681/.
Driscoll LL, Gibson AM, Hieb A. 2009. Chronic postnatal DE-71 exposure: effects on learning, attention and thyroxine levels. Neurotoxicol Teratol 31((2)):76-84 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19068229/.
Engel SM, Miodovnik A, Canfield RL, Zhu C, Silva MJ, Calafat AMet al. 2010. Prenatal phthalate exposure is associated with childhood behavior and executive functioning. Environ Health Perspect 118:565-571 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20106747/.
Eskenazi B, Marks AR, Bradman A, Harley K, Barr DB, Johnson Cet al. 2007. Organophosphate pesticide exposure and neurodevelopment in young Mexican-American children. Environ Health Perspect 115:792-798 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17520070/.
Fonnum F, Mariussen E, Reistad T. 2006. Molecular mechanisms involved in the toxic effects of polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) and brominated flame retardants (BFRs). J Toxicol Environ Health A 69((1–2)):21-35 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16291560/.
Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Auinger P, Hornung R, Epstein JN, Braun Jet al. 2009. Association of tobacco and lead exposures with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 124((6)):e1054-e1063 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19933729/.
Garavan H, Morgan RE, Levitsky DA, Hermer-Vazquez L, Strupp BJ. 2000. Enduring effects of early lead exposure: Evidence for a specific deficit in associative ability. Neurotoxicol Teratol 22((2)):151-164 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10758344/.
Gilbert SG, Rice DC. 1987. Low-level lifetime lead exposure produces behavioral toxicity (spatial discrimination reversal) in adult monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 91((3)):484-490 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3424377/.
Ha M, Kwon HJ, Lim MH, Jee YK, Hong YC, Leem JHet al. 2009. Low blood levels of lead and mercury and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity in children: a report of the Children’s Health and Environment Research (CHEER). Neurotoxicology 30((1)):31-36 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19100765/.
Hastings L, Zenick H, Succop P, Sun TJ, Sekeres R. 1984. Relationship between hematopoietic parameters and behavioral measures in lead-exposed rats. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 73((3)):416-422 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/6539005/.
Hauser R, Calafat AM. 2005. Phthalates and human health. Occup Environ Med 62((11)):806-818 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16234408/.
Hilson JA, Strupp BJ. 1997. Analyses of response patterns clarify lead effects in olfactory reversal and extradimensional shift tasks: assessment of inhibitory control, associative ability, and memory. Behav Neurosci 111((3)):532-542 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9189268/.
Hoffman K, Webster TF, Weisskopf MG, Weinberg J, Vieira VM. 2010. In press Exposure to polyfluoroalkyl chemicals and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in U.S. children 12–15 years of age. Environ Health Perspect 118:1762-1767 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20551004/.
Holene E, Nafstad I, Skaare JU, Krogh H, Sagvolden T. 1999. Behavioural effects in female rats of postnatal exposure to sub-toxic doses of polychlorinated biphenyl congener 153. Acta Paediatr Suppl 88((429)):55-63 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10419233/.
Holene E, Nafstad I, Skaare JU, Sagvolden T. 1998. Behavioural hyperactivity in rats following postnatal exposure to sub-toxic doses of polychlorinated biphenyl congeners 153 and 126. Behav Brain Res 94((1)):213-224 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9708851/.
Izaki Y, Takita M, Akema T. 2008. Specific role of the posterior dorsal hippocampus-prefrontal cortex in short-term working memory. Eur J Neurosci 27((11)):3029-3034 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18540879/.
Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. 2003. Prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and attention at school age. J Pediatr 143((6)):780-788 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14657828/.
Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Humphrey HE. 1990. Effects of in utero exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and related contaminants on cognitive functioning in young children. J Pediatr 116((1)):38-45 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2104928/.
Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Padgett RJ, Brumitt GA, Billings RL. 1992. Effects of prenatal PCB exposure on cognitive processing efficiency and sustained attention. Dev Psychol 28((2)):297-306.
Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL. 2006. Breast feeding and intelligence in children. BMJ 333((7575)):929-930 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17082524/.
Jensen AA, Leffers H. 2008. Emerging endocrine disruptors: perfluoroalkylated substances. Int J Androl 31((2)):161-169 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18315716/.
Jones DC, Miller GW. 2008. The effects of environmental neurotoxicants on the dopaminergic system: a possible role in drug addiction. Biochem Pharmacol 76((5)):569-581 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18555207/.
Jones RL, Homa DM, Meyer PA, Brody PA, Caldwell KC, Pirkle JLet al. 2009. Trends in blood lead levels and blood lead testing among US children aged 1 to 5 years, 1988–2004. Pediatrics 123((3)):e376-e385 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19254973/.
Kim BN, Cho SC, Kim Y, Shin MS, Yoo HJ, Kim JWet al. 2009. Phthalates exposure and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in school-age children. Biol Psychiatry 66((10)):958-963 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19748073/.
Kishi R, Ikeda T, Miyake H, Uchino E, Tsuzuki T, Inoue K. 1983. Effects of low lead exposure on neuro-behavioral function in the rat. Arch Environ Health 38((1)):25-33 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/6830315/.
Kordas K, Canfield RL, Lopez P, Rosado JL, Vargas GG, Cebrian MEet al. 2006. Deficits in cognitive function and achievement in Mexican first-graders with low blood lead concentrations. Environ Res 100((3)):371-386 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16169549/.
Kostyniak PJ, Hansen LG, Widholm JJ, Fitzpatrick RD, Olson JR, Helferich JLet al. 2005. Formulation and characterization of an experimental PCB mixture designed to mimic human exposure from contaminated fish. Toxicol Sci 88((2)):400-411 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16177234/.
Lanphear BP, Hornung R, Khoury J, Yolton K, Baghurst P, Bellinger DCet al. 2005. Low-level environmental lead exposure and children’s intellectual function: an international pooled analysis. Environ Health Perspect 113:894-899 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16002379/.
Letz R. 1994. NES2 Test User’s Manual. Version 6.6 Winchester, MA Neurobehavioral Systems.
Levin ED, Bowman RE. 1986. Long-term lead effects on the Hamilton search task and delayed alternation in monkeys. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 8((3)):219-224 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3736749/.
Levin ED, Bowman RE. 1988. Long-term effects of chronic postnatal lead exposure on delayed spatial alternation in monkeys. Neurotoxicol Teratol 10((6)):505-510 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3244342/.
Levin ED, Bowman RE, Wegert S, Vuchetich J. 1987. Psychopharmacological investigations of a lead-induced long-term cognitive deficit in monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 91((3)):334-341 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3104955/.
Levin ED, Schantz SL, Bowman RE. 1988. Delayed spatial alternation deficits resulting from perinatal PCB exposure in monkeys. Arch Toxicol 62((4)):267-273 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3149182/.
Lilienthal H, Neuf M, Munoz C, Winneke G. 1990. Behavioral effects of pre- and postnatal exposure to a mixture of low chlorinated PCBs in rats. Fundam Appl Toxicol 15((3)):457-467 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2124194/.
Longnecker MP, Wolff MS, Gladen BC, Brock JW, Grandjean P, Jacobson JLet al. 2003. Comparison of polychlorinated biphenyl levels across studies of human neurodevelopment. Environ Health Perspect 111:65-70 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12515680/.
Marcovitch S, Zelazo PD. 2009. A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Dev Sci 12((1)):1-18 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19120405/.
Mariussen E, Fonnum F. 2003. The effect of brominated flame retardants on neurotransmitter uptake into rat brain synaptosomes and vesicles. Neurochem Int 43((4–5)):533-542 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12742101/.
Marks AR, Harley K, Bradman A, Kogut K, Barr DB, Johnson Cet al. 2010. Organophosphate pesticide exposure and attention in young Mexican-American children: The CHAMACOS Study. Environ Health Perspect 118:1768-1774 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/21126939/.
Mele PC, Bowman RE, Levin ED. 1986. Behavioral evaluation of perinatal PCB exposure in rhesus monkeys: fixed-interval performance and reinforcement-omission. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 8((2)):131-138 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3086763/.
Mele PC, Bushnell PJ, Bowman RE. 1984. Prolonged behavioral effects of early postnatal lead exposure in rhesus monkeys: fixed-interval responding and interactions with scopolamine and pentobarbital. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 6((2)):129-135 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/6472557/.
Meyer PA, Brown MJ, Falk H. 2008. Global approach to reducing lead exposure and poisoning. Mutat Res 659((1–2)):166-175 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18436472/.
Milar KS, Krigman MR, Grant LD. 1981. Effects of neonatal lead exposure on memory in rats. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 3((3)):369-373 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/7290292/.
Min JY, Min KB, Cho SI, Kim R, Sakong J, Paek D. 2007. Neurobehavioral function in children with low blood lead concentrations. Neurotoxicology 28((2)):421-425 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16644013/.
Minder B, Das-Smaal EA, Brand EF, Orlebeke JF. 1994. Exposure to lead and specific attentional problems in schoolchildren. J Learn Disabil 27((6)):393-399 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/7519655/.
Minder B, Das-Smaal EA, Orlebeke JF. 1998. Cognition in children does not suffer from very low lead exposure. J Learn Disabil 31((5)):494-502 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9763778/.
Monsell S. 2003. Task switching. Trends Cogn Sci 7((3)):134-140 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12639695/.
Morgan RE, Garavan H, Smith EG, Driscoll LL, Levitsky DA, Strupp BJ. 2001. Early lead exposure produces lasting changes in sustained attention, response initiation, and reactivity to errors. Neurotoxicol Teratol 23((6)):519-531 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11792522/.
Newland MC, Yezhou S, Logdberg B, Berlin M. 1994. Prolonged behavioral effects of in utero exposure to lead or methyl mercury: reduced sensitivity to changes in reinforcement contingencies during behavioral transitions and in steady state. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 126((1)):6-15 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8184434/.
Nigg JT. 2006. What Causes ADHD? Understanding What Goes Wrong and Why New York Guilford.
Nigg JT, Knottnerus GM, Martel MM, Nikolas M, Cavanagh K, Karmaus Wet al. 2008. Low blood lead levels associated with clinically diagnosed attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and mediated by weak cognitive control. Biol Psychiatry 63((3)):325-331 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17868654/.
Nigg J, Nikolas M, Beauchaine TP, Hinshaw SP. 2008. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child and Adolescent Psychopathology Hoboken, NJ John Wiley & Sons 301.
Nigg JT, Nikolas M, Knottnerus GM, Cavanagh K, Friderici K. 2010. Confirmation and extension of association of blood lead with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and ADHD symptom domains at population-typical exposure levels. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 51((1)):58-65 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19941632/.
Overmann SR. 1977. Behavioral effects of asymptomatic lead exposure during neonatal development in rats. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 41((3)):459-471 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/918981/.
Pennington BF, McGrath LM, Rosenberg J, Barnard H, Smith SD, Willcutt EGet al. 2009. Gene × environment interactions in reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev Psychol 45((1)):77-89 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19209992/.
Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. 2007. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry 164((6)):942-948 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17541055/.
Rauh VA, Garfinkel R, Perera FP, Andrews HF, Hoepner L, Barr DBet al. 2006. Impact of prenatal chlorpyrifos exposure on neurodevelopment in the first 3 years of life among inner-city children. Pediatrics 118((6)):e1845-e1859 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17116700/.
Rice DC. 1985a. Chronic low-lead exposure from birth produces deficits in discrimination reversal in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 77((2)):201-210 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/4038826/.
Rice DC, Seiden LS, Balster RL. 1985b. Effect of lead on schedule-controlled behavior in monkeys. Behavioral Pharmacology: The Current Status New York Alan R. Liss 473-486.
Rice DC. 1988. Schedule-controlled behavior in infant and juvenile monkeys exposed to lead from birth. Neurotoxicology 9((1)):75-87 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3393304/.
Rice DC. 1990. Lead-induced behavioral impairment on a spatial discrimination reversal task in monkeys exposed during different periods of development. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 106((2)):327-333 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2256120/.
Rice DC. 1992. Behavioral effects of lead in monkeys tested during infancy and adulthood. Neurotoxicol Teratol 14((4)):235-245 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/1522828/.
Rice DC. 1993. Lead-induced changes in learning: evidence for behavioral mechanisms from experimental animal studies. Neurotoxicology 14((2–3)):167-178 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8247391/.
Rice DC. 1996. Behavioral effects of lead: commonalities between experimental and epidemiologic data. Environ Health Perspect 104((suppl 2)):337-351 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9182041/.
Rice DC. 1997. Effect of postnatal exposure to a PCB mixture in monkeys on multiple fixed interval-fixed ratio performance. Neurotoxicol Teratol 19((6)):429-434 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9392778/.
Rice DC. 1998. Effects of postnatal exposure of monkeys to a PCB mixture on spatial discrimination reversal and DRL performance. Neurotoxicol Teratol 20((4)):391-400 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9697965/.
Rice DC. 2000. Parallels between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and behavioral deficits produced by neurotoxic exposure in monkeys. Environ Health Perspect 108((suppl 3)):405-408 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10852836/.
Rice DC, Gilbert SG. 1985. Low lead exposure from birth produces behavioral toxicity (DRL) in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 80((3)):421-426 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3839945/.
Rice DC, Gilbert SG. 1990a. Lack of sensitive period for lead-induced behavioral impairment on a spatial delayed alternation task in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 103((2)):364-373 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2330594/.
Rice DC, Gilbert SG. 1990b. Sensitive periods for lead-induced behavioral impairment (nonspatial discrimination reversal) in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 102((1)):101-109 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2296763/.
Rice DC, Gilbert SG, Willes RF. 1979. Neonatal low-level lead exposure in monkeys: Locomotor activity, schedule-controlled behavior, and the effects of amphetamine. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 51((3)):503-513 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/120039/.
Rice DC, Hayward S. 1997. Effects of postnatal exposure to a PCB mixture in monkeys on nonspatial discrimination reversal and delayed alternation performance. Neurotoxicology 18((2)):479-494 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9291496/.
Rice DC, Hayward S. 1999. Effects of postnatal exposure of monkeys to a PCB mixture on concurrent random interval-random interval and progressive ratio performance. Neurotoxicol Teratol 21((1)):47-58 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10023801/.
Rice DC, Karpinski KF. 1988. Lifetime low-level lead exposure produces deficits in delayed alternation in adult monkeys. Neurotoxicol Teratol 10((3)):207-214 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3211098/.
Rice DC, Willes RF. 1979. Neonatal low-level lead exposure in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): effect on two-choice non-spatial form discrimination. J Environ Pathol Toxicol 2((4)):1195-1203 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/109561/.
Richter CA, Birnbaum LS, Farabollini F, Newbold RR, Rubin BS, Talsness CEet al. 2007. In vivo effects of bisphenol A in laboratory rodent studies. Reprod Toxicol 24((2)):199-224 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17683900/.
Ris MD, Dietrich KN, Succop PA, Berger OG, Bornschein RL. 2004. Early exposure to lead and neuropsychological outcome in adolescence. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 10((2)):261-270 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15012846/.
Robbins TW. 2002. The 5-choice serial reaction time task: behavioural pharmacology and functional neurochemistry. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 163((3–4)):362-380 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12373437/.
Roy A, Bellinger D, Hu H, Schwartz J, Ettinger AS, Wright ROet al. 2009. Lead exposure and behavior among young children in Chennai, India. Environ Health Perspect 117:1607-1611 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20019913/.
Roze E, Meijer L, Bakker A, Van Braeckel KN, Sauer PJ, Bos AF. 2009. Prenatal exposure to organohalogens, including brominated flame retardants, influences motor, cognitive, and behavioral performance at school age. Environ Health Perspect 117:1953-1958 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20049217/.
Sable HJ, Eubig PA, Powers BE, Wang VC, Schantz SL. 2009. Developmental exposure to PCBs and/or MeHg: effects on a differential reinforcement of low rates (DRL) operant task before and after amphetamine drug challenge. Neurotoxicol Teratol 31((3)):149-158 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19344642/.
Sable HJ, Powers BE, Wang VC, Widholm JJ, Schantz SL. 2006. Alterations in DRH and DRL performance in rats developmentally exposed to an environmental PCB mixture. Neurotoxicol Teratol 28((5)):548-556 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16930942/.
Sable HJK, Schantz SL, Levin ED, Buccafusco JJ. 2006. Executive function following developmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs): what animal models have told us. Animal Models of Cognitive Impairment Boca Raton, FL CRC Press 147-167.
Sagiv SK, Thurston SW, Bellinger DC, Tolbert PE, Altshul LM, Korrick SA. 2010. Prenatal organochlorine exposure and behaviors associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in school-aged children. Am J Epidemiol 171((5)):593-601 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20106937/.
Sagvolden T. 2000. Behavioral validation of the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD). Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24((1)):31-39 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10654658/.
Sagvolden T, Aase H, Zeiner P, Berger D. 1998. Altered reinforcement mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain Res 94((1)):61-71 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9708840/.
Samsam TE, Hunter DL, Bushnell PJ. 2005. Effects of chronic dietary and repeated acute exposure to chlorpyrifos on learning and sustained attention in rats. Toxicol Sci 87((2)):460-468 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16033991/.
Schantz SL, Levin ED, Bowman RE, Heironimus MP, Laughlin NK. 1989. Effects of perinatal PCB exposure on discrimination-reversal learning in monkeys. Neurotoxicol Teratol 11((3)):243-250 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2502707/.
Schantz SL, Moshtaghian J, Ness DK. 1995. Spatial learning deficits in adult rats exposed to ortho-substituted PCB congeners during gestation and lactation. Fundam Appl Toxicol 26((1)):117-126 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/7657055/.
Schantz SL, Seo BW, Wong PW, Pessah IN. 1997. Long-term effects of developmental exposure to 2,2′,3,5′,6-pentachlorobiphenyl (PCB 95) on locomotor activity, spatial learning and memory and brain ryanodine binding. Neurotoxicology 18((2)):457-467 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9291494/.
Seegal RF, Brosch KO, Okoniewski RJ. 1997. Effects of in utero and lactational exposure of the laboratory rat to 2,4,2′,4′- and 3,4,3′,4′-tetrachlorobiphenyl on dopamine function. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 146((1)):95-103 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9299601/.
Seegal RF, Bush B, Brosch KO. 1991. Comparison of effects of Aroclors 1016 and 1260 on non-human primate catecholamine function. Toxicology 66((2)):145-163 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2014516/.
Sjodin A, Wong LY, Jones RS, Park A, Zhang Y, Hodge Cet al. 2008. Serum concentrations of polybrominated diphenyl ethers (PBDEs) and polybrominated biphenyl (PBB) in the United States population: 2003–2004. Environ Sci Technol 42((4)):1377-1384 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18351120/.
Sloan HL, Good M, Dunnett SB. 2006. Double dissociation between hippocampal and prefrontal lesions on an operant delayed matching task and a water maze reference memory task. Behav Brain Res 171((1)):116-126 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16677723/.
Smith AK, Mick E, Faraone SV. 2009. Advances in genetic studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 11((2)):143-148 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19302768/.
Stangle DE, Smith DR, Beaudin SA, Strawderman MS, Levitsky DA, Strupp BJ. 2007. Succimer chelation improves learning, attention, and arousal regulation in lead-exposed rats but produces lasting cognitive impairment in the absence of lead exposure. Environ Health Perspect 115:201-209 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17384765/.
Stanton ME, Mundy WR, Ward T, Dulchinos V, Barry CC. 1994. Time-dependent effects of acute chlorpyrifos administration on spatial delayed alternation and cholinergic neurochemistry in weanling rats. Neurotoxicology 15((1)):201-208 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/7522308/.
Stewart P, Fitzgerald S, Reihman J, Gump B, Lonky E, Darvill Tet al. 2003. Prenatal PCB exposure, the corpus callosum, and response inhibition. Environ Health Perspect 111:1670-1677 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14527849/.
Stewart P, Reihman J, Gump B, Lonky E, Darvill T, Pagano J. 2005. Response inhibition at 8 and 9 1/2 years of age in children prenatally exposed to PCBs. Neurotoxicol Teratol 27((6)):771-780 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16198536/.
Stewart PW, Lonky E, Reihman J, Pagano J, Gump BB, Darvill T. 2008. The relationship between prenatal PCB exposure and intelligence (IQ) in 9-year-old children. Environ Health Perspect 116:1416-1422 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18941588/.
Stewart PW, Sargent DM, Reihman J, Gump BB, Lonky E, Darvill Tet al. 2006. Response inhibition during differential reinforcement of low rates (DRL) schedules may be sensitive to low-level polychlorinated biphenyl, methylmercury, and lead exposure in children. Environ Health Perspect 114:1923-1929 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17185286/.
Stiles KM, Bellinger DC. 1993. Neuropsychological correlates of low-level lead exposure in school-age children: a prospective study. Neurotoxicol Teratol 15((1)):27-35 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8459785/.
Surkan PJ, Zhang A, Trachtenberg F, Daniel DB, McKinlay S, Bellinger DC. 2007. Neuropsychological function in children with blood lead levels < 10 microg/dL. Neurotoxicology 28((6)):1170-1177 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17868887/.
Swanson JM, Kinsbourne M, Nigg J, Lanphear B, Stefanatos GA, Volkow Net al. 2007. Etiologic subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: brain imaging, molecular genetic and environmental factors and the dopamine hypothesis. Neuropsychol Rev 17((1)):39-59 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17318414/.
Taylor MM, Crofton KM, MacPhail RC. 2002. Schedule-controlled behavior in rats exposed perinatally to the PCB mixture Aroclor 1254. Neurotoxicol Teratol 24((4)):511-518 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12127897/.
Vaidya CJ, Stollstorff M. 2008. Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: current status and working hypotheses. Dev Disabil Res Rev 14((4)):261-267 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19072750/.
Vandenberg LN, Chauhoud I, Heindel JJ, Padmanabhan V, Paumgartten FJ, Schoenfelder G. 2010. Urinary, circulating and tissue biomonitoring studies indicate widespread exposure to bisphenol A. Environ Health Perspect 118:1055-1070 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20338858/.
Vreugdenhil HJ, Lanting CI, Mulder PG, Boersma ER, Weisglas-Kuperus N. 2002. Effects of prenatal PCB and dioxin background exposure on cognitive and motor abilities in Dutch children at school age. J Pediatr 140((1)):48-56 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11815763/.
Vreugdenhil HJ, Mulder PG, Emmen HH, Weisglas-Kuperus N. 2004a. Effects of perinatal exposure to PCBs on neuropsychological functions in the Rotterdam cohort at 9 years of age. Neuropsychology 18((1)):185-193 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14744201/.
Vreugdenhil HJ, Van Zanten GA, Brocaar MP, Mulder PG, Weisglas-Kuperus N. 2004b. Prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and breastfeeding: opposing effects on auditory P300 latencies in 9-year-old Dutch children. Dev Med Child Neurol 46((6)):398-405 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15174531/.
Walkowiak J, Altmann L, Kramer U, Sveinsson K, Turfeld M, Weishoff-Houben Met al. 1998. Cognitive and sensorimotor functions in 6-year-old children in relation to lead and mercury levels: adjustment for intelligence and contrast sensitivity in computerized testing. Neurotoxicol Teratol 20((5)):511-521 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9761589/.
Walshaw PD, Alloy LB, Sabb FW. 2010. Executive function in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: in search of distinct phenotypic profiles. Neuropsychol Rev 20((1)):103-120 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20165924/.
Wang HL, Chen XT, Yang B, Ma FL, Wang S, Tang MLet al. 2008. Case–control study of blood lead levels and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Chinese children. Environ Health Perspect 116:1401-1406 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18941585/.
Widholm JJ, Clarkson GB, Strupp BJ, Crofton KM, Seegal RF, Schantz SL. 2001. Spatial reversal learning in Aroclor 1254-exposed rats: sex-specific deficits in associative ability and inhibitory control. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 174((2)):188-198 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11446834/.
Widholm JJ, Villareal S, Seegal RF, Schantz SL. 2004. Spatial alternation deficits following developmental exposure to Aroclor 1254 and/or methylmercury in rats. Toxicol Sci 82((2)):577-589 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15456922/.
Winstanley CA, Eagle DM, Robbins TW. 2006. Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: translation between clinical and preclinical studies. Clin Psychol Rev 26((4)):379-395 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16504359/.
Zahalka EA, Ellis DH, Goldey ES, Stanton ME, Lau C. 2001. Perinatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls Aroclor 1016 or 1254 did not alter brain catecholamines nor delayed alternation performance in Long-Evans rats. Brain Res Bull 55((4)):487-500 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11543949/.
Zenick H, Rodriquez W, Ward J, Elkington B. 1979. Deficits in fixed-interval performance following prenatal and postnatal lead exposure. Dev Psychobiol 12((5)):509-514 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/488533/.
Zuch CL, O’Mara DJ, Cory-Slechta DA. 1998. Low-level lead exposure selectively enhances dopamine overflow in nucleus accumbens: an in vivo electrochemistry time course assessment. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 150((1)):174-185 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9630467/.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Environmental Health Perspectives
Volume 118Issue 12December 2010
Pages: 1654 - 1667
PubMed: 20829149

History

Received: 18 December 2009
Accepted: 9 September 2010
Published online: 9 September 2010

Keywords

  1. ADHD
  2. attention
  3. executive function
  4. lead
  5. PCBs

Authors

Affiliations

Paul A. Eubig [email protected]
Department of Comparative Biosciences, College of Veterinary Medicine and
Andréa Aguiar
Department of Comparative Biosciences, College of Veterinary Medicine and
Susan L. Schantz
Department of Comparative Biosciences, College of Veterinary Medicine and
Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA

Notes

Address correspondence to P.A. Eubig, Department of Comparative Biosciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, 2001 S. Lincoln Ave., Urbana, IL 61802 USA. Telephone: (217) 333-6230. Fax: (217) 244-1652. E-mail: [email protected]
The authors declare they have no actual or potential competing financial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

About Article Metrics


Citations

Download citation

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click DOWNLOAD.

Cited by

  • Blood Lead Level among Chronic Kidney Diseases Patients Attended Medical City Hospital in Baghdad 2022, Iraqi Journal of Community Medicine, 10.4103/IRJCM.IRJCM_3_24, 37, 1, (6-10), (2024).
  • Shrinking the footprint of the criminal legal system through policies informed by psychology and neuroscience, Communications Psychology, 10.1038/s44271-024-00090-9, 2, 1, (2024).
  • Passive Air Sampling Networks Combined with Multivariate Statistics Reveal Widespread Non-Aroclor Polychlorinated Biphenyl Sources to the Canadian Atmosphere, ACS ES&T Air, 10.1021/acsestair.3c00081, 1, 6, (481-491), (2024).
  • The research landscape concerning environmental factors in neurodevelopmental disorders: Endocrine disrupters and pesticides—A review, Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 10.1016/j.yfrne.2024.101132, 73, (101132), (2024).
  • Blood lead levels in the general population and vulnerable sub-populations and related risk factors for lead exposure in Canada, Hygiene and Environmental Health Advances, 10.1016/j.heha.2024.100088, 9, (100088), (2024).
  • Effect of Lead Exposure and Lifestyle Factors on Methylation Index Markers Among Pb-Exposed Workers, Biological Trace Element Research, 10.1007/s12011-024-04270-w, (2024).
  • Identifying novel chemical-related susceptibility genes for five psychiatric disorders through integrating genome-wide association study and tissue-specific 3′aQTL annotation datasets, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 10.1007/s00406-023-01753-0, (2024).
  • The Neurotoxic and Neurobehavioral Effects of “Tartrazine E102” Alone and in Combination with Lead Nitrate on the Sprague Dawley Rat, International Conference on Advanced Intelligent Systems for Sustainable Development (AI2SD’2023), 10.1007/978-3-031-52388-5_34, (379-393), (2024).
  • Effects of Endocrine-Disrupting Chemicals on Human Health, Laboratory Medicine Online, 10.47429/lmo.2023.13.3.129, 13, 3, (129-140), (2023).
  • Blood Lead Level and Renal Function: Sample of Iraqi Patients Attending Baghdad Teaching Hospital in 2022, Mustansiriya Medical Journal, 10.4103/mj.mj_15_23, 22, 1, (124-127), (2023).
  • See more

View Options

View options

PDF

View PDF

Get Access

Restore your content access

Enter your email address to restore your content access:

Note: This functionality works only for purchases done as a guest. If you already have an account, log in to access the content to which you are entitled.

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Copy the content Link

Share on social media