Skip to main content
Open access
Research
26 October 2015

Associations of Cognitive Function Scores with Carbon Dioxide, Ventilation, and Volatile Organic Compound Exposures in Office Workers: A Controlled Exposure Study of Green and Conventional Office Environments

Publication: Environmental Health Perspectives
Volume 124, Issue 6
Pages 805 - 812

Abstract

Background:

The indoor built environment plays a critical role in our overall well-being because of both the amount of time we spend indoors (~90%) and the ability of buildings to positively or negatively influence our health. The advent of sustainable design or green building strategies reinvigorated questions regarding the specific factors in buildings that lead to optimized conditions for health and productivity.

Objective:

We simulated indoor environmental quality (IEQ) conditions in “Green” and “Conventional” buildings and evaluated the impacts on an objective measure of human performance: higher-order cognitive function.

Methods:

Twenty-four participants spent 6 full work days (0900–1700 hours) in an environmentally controlled office space, blinded to test conditions. On different days, they were exposed to IEQ conditions representative of Conventional [high concentrations of volatile organic compounds (VOCs)] and Green (low concentrations of VOCs) office buildings in the United States. Additional conditions simulated a Green building with a high outdoor air ventilation rate (labeled Green+) and artificially elevated carbon dioxide (CO2) levels independent of ventilation.

Results:

On average, cognitive scores were 61% higher on the Green building day and 101% higher on the two Green+ building days than on the Conventional building day (p < 0.0001). VOCs and CO2 were independently associated with cognitive scores.

Conclusions:

Cognitive function scores were significantly better under Green+ building conditions than in the Conventional building conditions for all nine functional domains. These findings have wide-ranging implications because this study was designed to reflect conditions that are commonly encountered every day in many indoor environments.

Citation:

Allen JG, MacNaughton P, Satish U, Santanam S, Vallarino J, Spengler JD. 2016. Associations of cognitive function scores with carbon dioxide, ventilation, and volatile organic compound exposures in office workers: a controlled exposure study of green and conventional office environments. Environ Health Perspect 124:805–812; http://dx.doi.org/10.1289/ehp.1510037

Introduction

The increasing cost of energy in the 1970s led to a change in building practices throughout the United States as buildings were increasingly constructed to be airtight and energy efficient. These changes are reflected in decreasing air exchange rates in homes and office buildings. For homes, beginning in this time period, typical air exchange rates began decreasing from approximately 1 air change per hour (ACH) to approximately 0.5 ACH [Chan et al. 2003; Hodgson et al. 2000; American Society of Heating, Refrigerating, and Air-Conditioning Engineers (ASHRAE) 2013b].
Homes built since 2000 are designed to be even more energy efficient and therefore can be even tighter [0.1–0.2 ACH (Allen et al. 2012; ASHRAE 2013b)]. The > 100-year story of ventilation in buildings is complicated and was neatly summarized recently by Persily (2015). Persily describes the original ASHRAE 62 standard, issued in 1973, and the many subsequent iterations (e.g., ASHRAE 62.1 applies to commercial buildings), demonstrating the evolving nature of our understanding regarding the relationship between ventilation rate and acceptable indoor air quality. Similarly to the history of home ventilation, commercial ventilation requirements were lowered in the early 1980s, largely as an energy-conservation measure (Persily 2015).
With such design changes comes the potential for negative consequences to indoor environmental quality (IEQ) because decreased ventilation can lead to increased concentration of indoor pollutants. Building-related illnesses and sick building syndrome (SBS) were first reported in the 1980s as ventilation rates decreased (Riesenberg and Arehart-Treichel 1986), with significant annual costs and productivity losses due to health symptoms attributable to the indoor environment (Fisk and Rosenfeld 1997). A few factors of the indoor and work environments have been found to be associated with occupant health. These factors include environmental measures, such as humidity; building factors, such as ventilation rate; workspace factors, such as the presence of chemical-emitting materials; and personal factors, such as job stress, allergies, and sex (Mendell 1993; Wargocki et al. 2000; Bornehag et al. 2005; Hedge 2009; Hedge and Gaygen 2010; Nishihara et al. 2014).
The IEQ problems that arose from conventional buildings with a tight envelope contributed to the advent of sustainable design or “green” building rating systems [e.g., U.S. Green Building Council’s (USGBC’s) Leadership in Energy and Environmental Design (LEED®)]. These rating systems aim to reduce the environmental footprint of buildings and to improve occupant health by providing design credits to new and existing buildings for adopting green design, operation, and maintenance. Different levels of ratings for the building are then awarded based on the number of acquired credits (e.g., silver, gold, platinum) (USGBC 2014). Many design credits are aimed at energy efficiency and environmental performance but also include guidelines for improving ventilation and filtration, using low-emitting materials, controlling indoor chemical and pollutant sources, improving thermal and lighting conditions, and offering daylight views to building occupants (USGBC 2014). Compared with conventional buildings, environmental measurements in green buildings show lower concentrations of several key pollutants including particles, nitrogen dioxide, volatile organic compounds (VOCs), and allergens (Colton et al. 2014; Jacobs et al. 2015; Noris et al. 2013). However, these reductions generally did not extend to carbon dioxide (CO2) or air exchange rate, demonstrating the influence of energy efficiency on green building operation and design. Green buildings were associated with improved IEQ and have been associated with reductions in self-reported symptoms in people inhabiting the buildings and with improved productivity in home, school, and office settings (Colton et al. 2014; NRC 2007; Singh et al. 2010). However, an important limitation of these studies is their reliance on subjective outcome measures, such as surveys, that have the potential for bias because participants are aware of their status (i.e., green or control). To date, we know of no studies that have been conducted in green buildings where participants were blinded to their building condition (Allen et al. 2015).
We designed this study to objectively quantify the impact of indoor environment on higher-order cognitive function, a driver of real-world productivity in office workers. We simulated low-VOC (“Green”) and high-VOC (“Conventional”) building conditions, both at the ASHRAE standard ventilation rate. Recognizing that technological advances in mechanical systems open the possibility of increasing ventilation rates without sacrificing energy efficiency, we also tested another building condition that introduced higher rates of ventilation to the Green building condition. This condition was labeled Green+. Last, we were motivated by the recent findings by Satish et al. (2012) that CO2 may be a direct pollutant and not just an indicator of ventilation; therefore, we assessed cognitive function after a full-workday exposure to CO2 while holding other variables constant.

Methods

Study Design

This study was undertaken in a controlled office environment to estimate the effects of several indoor environmental quality parameters on an objective measure of cognitive function. We used a double-blinded study design that included repeated measures of cognitive function on the same individual, characterization of potential confounding IEQ variables, and midweek testing to avoid Monday/Friday effects. All participants received the same exposures on each day, with exposures varying each day.

Study Population

Twenty-four professional-grade employees (architects, designers, programmers, engineers, creative marketing professionals, managers) in the Syracuse, New York, area participated in a 6-day longitudinal study of cognitive performance and building conditions (Table 1). Six additional people were originally recruited as backups but were not enrolled in the study. Participants were recruited through emails to local businesses. The study population was restricted to nonsensitive persons by excluding current smokers and people with asthma (because of testing indoor-air quality), claustrophobia, and schizophrenia (because this was an experiment where participants were required to remain in the laboratory). The participants were relocated to the Willis H. Carrier Total Indoor Environmental Quality (TIEQ) Laboratory at the Syracuse Center of Excellence (CoE) for 6 days over the course of 2 weeks in November of 2014. The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health Institutional Review Board (IRB). SUNY Upstate Medical and Syracuse University ceded their review to Harvard’s IRB. All participants signed informed consent documents and were compensated with $800.
Table 1 Participant demographics.
Categoryn%
Sex
Male1042
Female1458
Age
20–30833
31–40312
41–50625
51–60417
61–70312
Ethnicity
White/Caucasian2292
Black or African American14
Latino14
Highest level of schooling
High school graduate14
Some college28
College degree1354
Graduate degree833
Job category
Managerial521
Professional1563
Technical14
Secretarial or clerical14
Other28
Participants reported to the CoE on Tuesday, Wednesday, and Thursday, at 0900 hours, for 2 consecutive weeks. The CoE has two nearly identical office environments located adjacent to one another as part of the TIEQ Lab, each with 12 cubicles. The rooms are similarly constructed and have identical building materials (e.g., carpeting, cubicles, painting, computers). Environmental conditions, described in the following sections, were designed to be consistent in the two rooms. On the first day, the participants were randomly assigned to cubicles in the TIEQ Lab for the duration of the study. Participants were requested to spend the entire work day in the simulated office environments performing their normal work activities. They were provided with computers, internet access, and an area for private telephone calls and printing. A 45-min lunch break was given between 1200 and 1245 hours (Room 1) or 1215 and 1300 hours (Room 2). A limited selection of food was provided, served, and eaten in a room adjacent to the two simulated office environment rooms. Participants then returned to the simulated office environment to continue their work. Cognitive testing was initiated at 1500 hours each day, after which the participants completed the daily surveys and left the TIEQ Lab. Participants were blinded to test conditions, as were the analysts performing the cognitive function assessment. Participants were not given any instructions regarding how to spend their time in the evenings or on the Mondays before starting the test period.

Indoor Environment Simulation

The different environmental simulations in the TIEQ Lab on each day were designed to evaluate commonly encountered conditions and guidance values (Table 2). The three test parameters that were experimentally controlled were ventilation with outdoor air, CO2, and VOCs. We selected two outdoor air ventilation rates for this study: 20 cfm/person and 40 cfm/person. LEED® specifies that mechanically ventilated spaces must meet ventilation rates under ASHRAE 62.1 or the local equivalent, whichever is more stringent (USGBC 2014; ASHRAE 2013a). Many local building codes use the previous ASHRAE standard of 20 cfm/person, which corresponds to an indoor CO2 concentration of 945 ppm. Therefore, 20 cfm/person was the ventilation rate we used for the Green and Conventional simulation days because it reflects the minimum required ventilation rate for both green buildings (through LEED®) and conventional buildings (through ASHRAE). We also sought to evaluate the impact of a doubling of that minimum rate to 40 cfm/person (labeled Green+ days), which corresponds to an approximate steady-state CO2 concentration of 550 ppm. To ensure blinding, air movement was maintained at 40 cfm per person on all study days, with 100% outdoor air ventilation used on Green+ days and moderate and high CO2 days, and a mix of 50% outdoor air and 50% recirculated air used on the Green and Conventional days to achieve 20 cfm outdoor air ventilation per person.
Table 2 Average indoor environmental conditions simulated in each room of the TIEQ lab.
VariableDay 1 Green+Day 2 Moderate CO2Day 3 High CO2Day 4 GreenDay 5 ConventionalDay 6 Green+
Date4 November5 November6 November11 November12 November13 November
Day of the weekTuesdayWednesdayThursdayTuesdayWednesdayThursday
Room502503502503502503502503502503502503
Experimental parameters
CO2 (ppm)5636099069621,4001,420761b726b969921486488
Outdoor air ventilation (cfm/person)a404040404040202020204040
TVOCs (μg/m3)43.438.538.228.632.229.848.543.550666655.814.9
Other environmental parameters
Temperature (°C)23.924.522.423.921.322.022.923.721.822.520.721.3
Relative humidity (%)31.030.434.231.638.738.334.333.339.638.327.826.8
NO2 (μg/m3)57.958.953.254.160.858.451.345.654.650.856.555.5
O3 (μg/m3)3.4221.214.413.01.370.006.852381.711.374.116.85
PM2.5 (μg/m3)2.383.493.352.582.972.421.261.831.681.341.261.38
Noise (dB)51.349.949.748.852.548.849.648.751.148.850.549.2
Illuminance (mV)2.952.702.892.832.312.043.112.932.742.512.392.28
Irradiance (mV)9.078.769.459.376.006.059.909.608.308.146.706.82
Abbreviations: TIEQ, Total Indoor Environmental Quality; TVOCs, total volatile organic compounds. aA constant air flow rate of 40 cfm/person was maintained on all study days, with 100% outdoor air used on days 1, 2, 3, and 6 and 50% outdoor air and 50% recirculated air used to achieve an outdoor air ventilation rate of 20 cfm/person on days 4 and 5. bAverage concentration from 1400 to 1700 hours was 926 ppm, but lower CO2 concentrations in the morning hours during the approach to steady state led to a lower average CO2 concentration.
For the assessment of the independent association of CO2 concentration with cognitive function, the outdoor air ventilation rate was held constant at 40 cfm/person while CO2 was added to the chambers to reach three steady-state CO2 concentrations. The first target was 550 ppm (Green+, Days 1 and 6). The second target, 945 ppm, was selected to reflect a level that would be expected at the previously described ASHRAE minimum recommended ventilation rate of 20 cfm outdoor air/person. The third target, 1,400 ppm, was selected to represent a higher, but not uncommon, concentration of CO2 found in indoor environments [1,400 ppm is the maximum observed 8-hr time-weighted-average CO2 concentration in the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) BASE data set (U.S. EPA 1998)]. On Days 2 and 3, when the independent effects of CO2 were tested, CO2 was added from a cylinder of ultra-pure CO2 (≥ 99.9999% pure) to the TIEQ Lab supply air at a rate needed to maintain steady-state CO2 concentrations of 945 ppm and 1,400 ppm. Because CO2 concentrations are affected by occupancy and mixing impact concentrations, a technician monitored CO2 in real time and adjusted the emission rate accordingly to maintain constant CO2 concentrations. During Days 4 and 5 (Green and Conventional), injection of pure CO2 was not needed to reach the target CO2 concentrations because of the reduced outdoor ventilation rate. A protocol was established to ensure participant safety in the event that there were unexpected deviations. CO2 was monitored in real time at a high spatial resolution in the test rooms using three different and independently calibrated monitors. A technician seated next to the CO2 shut-off valves monitored the CO2 concentrations during the entire test period. The protocol called for immediately canceling the testing if CO2 concentrations exceeded preset thresholds that were well below occupational health limits [2,500 ppm, one-half of the threshold limit value set by the American Conference of Governmental Industrial Hygienists (ACGIH 2015)]. No deviations from protocol occurred during the study.
The TIEQ Lab was constructed with low-VOC materials, and low levels of VOCs were confirmed by pretesting (Table 3). To simulate a conventional office space with elevated VOCs, we placed VOC sources in the diffuser that supplied air to each cubicle area before the participants arrived on Day 5. We selected a target total VOC (TVOC) level of 500 μg/m3 based on the LEED® Indoor Air Quality Assessment credit limit, as measured using U.S. EPA method TO-15 (USGBC 2014). The diffusers were built into the floor of the TIEQ Lab, and there were no visible indicators of these sources for the participants to observe. We selected a mix of nonodoriferous sources to simulate VOC-emitting materials that are commonly found in office buildings and that covered four indoor VOC source categories including building materials [56 in2 (360 cm2) exposed edge melamine, 56 in2 (360 cm2) exposed edge particle board, 64 in2 (415 cm2) vinyl mat], adhesives [80 in2 (520 cm2) duct tape, 80 in2 (520 cm2) packing tape (exposed)], cleaning products [1 oz. (30 mL) multi-surface cleaner, 4 multi-surface wipes, 144 in2 (930 cm2) recently dry-cleaned cloth], and office supplies (4 dry erase markers, 1 open bottle of correction fluid).
Table 3 Speciated VOC concentrations (μg/m3) on each study day, averaged across rooms.
AnalyteCondition
BackgroundDay 1 Green+Day 2 Med. CO2Day 3 High CO2Day 4 GreenDay 5 ConventionalDay 6 Green+
VOCs
1,2,4-Trimethylbenzene0.30.2ND0.1ND0.50.1
2-Butanone2.50.70.70.81.11.10.6
2-Propanol1.01.21.13.11.2312.58.2
Acetone12.014.79.68.720.020.08.6
Benzene0.50.80.50.90.70.50.5
Carbon disulfide0.60.2NDNDNDND0.1
Carbon tetrachlorideND0.20.4ND0.2NDND
ChloroformND0.1NDNDND0.1ND
Chloromethane1.31.71.51.41.91.51.4
Cyclohexane0.20.30.40.50.10.40.3
Dichlorodifluoromethane2.52.62.92.72.92.42.5
Ethyl acetateNDNDNDND1.02.0ND
Ethylbenzene0.30.4ND0.30.20.10.1
Freon 1130.30.70.80.80.80.20.4
HeptaneND0.3ND0.3ND257.56.9
Hexane0.40.70.50.70.40.81.3
m,p-Xylene0.81.50.41.01.00.70.7
Methylene chloride0.50.30.60.50.30.40.4
o-Xylene0.30.4ND0.40.10.30.1
Styrene0.1NDNDNDNDND0.1
Tetrachloroethene3.70.9NDND0.90.60.2
TetrahydrofuranNDNDNDND0.20.10.2
Toluene2.42.11.41.92.21.92.9
trans-1,2-Dichloroethene19.08.812.66.210.321.88.7
TrichloroetheneNDNDNDNDNDND0.2
Trichlorofluoromethane1.31.21.61.41.51.11.2
Grand total50.040.135.031.446.9626.445.6
Aldehydes
2,5-DimethylbenzaldehydeNDNDNDNDNDNDND
Acetaldehyde1.03.73.23.15.47.32.1
BenzaldehydeNDNDNDNDND1.5ND
CrotonaldehydeNDNDNDNDNDNDND
Formaldehyde2.45.95.55.48.911.74.4
HexanaldehydeND0.80.8ND1.92.4ND
IsovaleraldehydeNDNDNDNDNDNDND
m,p-TolualdehydeNDNDNDNDNDNDND
n-Butyraldehyde1.12.71.42.32.82.42.0
o-TolualdehydeNDNDNDNDNDNDND
PropionaldehydeND0.71.2ND1.41.60.6
ValeraldehydeNDNDNDNDNDNDND
GlutaraldehydeND0.5NDND0.4NDND
o-PhthalaldehydeND65.157.770.041.638.476.8
Grand total4.679.469.880.962.465.385.8
Abbreviations: ND, non-detect; VOC, volatile organic compound.

Environmental Monitoring

The study team characterized the TIEQ Lab on each test day for a wide range of IEQ indicators: CO2, temperature, relative humidity, barometric pressure, sound levels, VOCs, aldehydes, nitrogen dioxide (NO2), ozone (O3), particulate matter ≤ 2.5 μm in diameter (PM2.5), and light. Netatmo Weather Stations were installed in each cubicle to measure temperature, humidity, carbon dioxide concentrations (parts per million), and sound levels (decibels) every 5 min for each participant. The stations were calibrated to 0 and 3,000 ppm CO2 using calibration gases and were validated using a calibrated TSI Q-Trak (model 7575). In addition, the Netatmos were tested with 400 and 1,000 ppm calibration gas at the end of the study to determine if the sensors drifted during the 2-week period. Duplicate measures of CO2 were collected in each room using a TSI Q-Trak model 7575 and two K-33 data loggers. Summa canisters were used to detect overall levels of 62 common VOCs in a randomly selected workstation in each room for each of the study days (Table 3). An additional sample was collected in a third randomly selected cubicle each day. Samples were analyzed by ALS Laboratories according to U.S. EPA method TO-15 (U.S. EPA 1999). Thirty-six VOCs were not detected in any of the samples.
In each room, a monitoring station was placed at the far end of the room from the entrance to monitor additional IEQ parameters. The station included a) a TSI SidePak AM510 personal aerosol monitor to measure PM2.5, b) an integrated filter sample for gravimetric analysis of PM2.5 and elemental composition, c) an 8-hr integrated active air sample (0.4 L/min flow rate) analyzed for 14 aldehydes by ALS Analytical Laboratories using U.S. EPA method TO-11 (U.S. EPA 1999), d) a passive NO2 badge [8-hr time-weighted average; model X-595, Assay Technology; Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA) method 182 (OSHA 1991)], e) a passive sampling badge for O3 [8-hr time-weighted average; model X-586, Assay Technology; OSHA Method 214 (OSHA 2008)], and f) illuminance and irradiance measures using an IL1400 radiometer/powermeter with SEL-033/Y/W and SEL-033/F/W detectors. VOC, aldehyde, NO2, O3, and integrated PM2.5 samples had at least one blank and one duplicate for every 10 samples. Samples were blank-corrected for analyses. All duplicate measures were within 15% of each other, and an average of the two was used for subsequent analyses.
An ambient air monitoring system was installed on the roof of the CoE to measure PM2.5, O3, and NO2 using the same procedures and equipment as the indoor stations to establish the potential influence of outdoor contaminants on the indoor environment. Outdoor temperature, humidity, solar radiation, and wind speed/direction data were obtained from the CoE weather station located on the roof of the building. Baseline (i.e., before occupancy) measurements of all IEQ parameters were collected in the TIEQ Lab 1 month before the study was performed.

Cognitive Function Assessment

The cognitive assessment was performed daily using the Strategic Management Simulation (SMS) software tool, which is a validated, computer-based test that has been designed to test the effectiveness of management-level employees through assessments of higher-order decision making (Streufert et al. 1988; Breuer and Satish 2003; Satish et al. 2004). At the start of the 1.5-hr test, participants were given a brief, 1-page description of the scenario that they were about to participate in during the test. They were then logged onto a standardized desktop computer station at the TIEQ Lab using a unique identifier. Participants were not allowed to use their own computers and were instructed to turn off all other devices before the assessment. The simulation was then initiated. Participants were exposed to diverse situations based on real-world equivalent challenges (e.g., handling a township in the role of a mayor or emergency coordinator). These scenarios are designed to capture participants’ standard response pattern. The software allows flexibility in approach; participants can choose to make a decision or form a plan at any time in response to any stimulus from the program. The absence of requirements or stated demands allows the participant the freedom to strategize and take initiative in his or her typical cognitive style. Based on the participant’s actions, plans, responses to incoming information, and use of prior actions and outcomes, the SMS software computes scores for nine cognitive factors (Table 4).
Table 4 Description of the cognitive domains tested.
Cognitive function domainaDescription
Basic Activity LevelOverall ability to make decisions at all times
Applied Activity LevelCapacity to make decisions that are geared toward overall goals
Focused Activity LevelCapacity to pay attention to situations at hand
Task OrientationCapacity to make specific decisions that are geared toward completion of tasks at hand
Crisis ResponseAbility to plan, stay prepared, and strategize under emergency conditions
Information SeekingCapacity to gather information as required from different available sources
Information UsageCapacity to use both provided information and information that has been gathered toward attaining overall goals
Breadth of ApproachCapacity to make decisions along multiple dimensions and use a variety of options and opportunities to attain goals
StrategyComplex thinking parameter that reflects the ability to use well-integrated solutions with the help of optimal use of information and planning
aSee Streufert et al. (1986) for detailed descriptions.
A technician trained in administering this test was present to provide standardized instructions and periodically answer any questions from participants. Parallel scenarios (i.e., equivalent scenarios) were used from one day to the next, which allows individuals to be retested without potential bias caused by experience and learning effects (Swezey et al. 1998). Parallel scenarios have correlation coefficients between 0.68 and 0.94 for the scores on these cognitive function domains (Streufert et al. 1988).

Statistical Analyses

Generalized additive mixed effect models were used to test associations between environmental exposures and cognitive function while controlling for the correlated nature of the repeat measures. In the model, the most specific exposure was assigned to each participant, whether it be cubicle-level (CO2), room-level (VOCs), or lab-level (ventilation). Participant ID was treated as a random intercept to control for confounding by individual characteristics. The residuals were normally distributed and homoscedastic for all models (data not shown). We used penalized splines to graphically assess linearity in the associations between environmental exposures and cognitive scores. SMS scores are often compared with normative data from other uses of the SMS software (e.g., Satish et al. 2012). Because we did not have access to normative data, we instead used our study population as the reference group. Based on the analysis, cognitive scores were normalized to Conventional (Table 5), Green (Figure 1), or Green+ (Figure 2) scores to allow for comparisons across cognitive function domains, each of which has a unique scale in its raw form. The scores were normalized for each cognitive domain by dividing all scores by the average score obtained during the normalizing condition. The statistical significance of our results was not affected by normalization. Given the multiple comparisons tested in this analysis, p-values < 0.001 were considered to be statistically significant after performing a Bonferroni correction. Analyses were performed using the open-source statistical package R v.3.0.0 (R Core Team 2015).
Table 5 Generalized additive mixed effect models testing the effect of IEQ condition and on cognitive scores, normalized to the “Conventional” condition, treating participant as a random intercept.
ConditionCognitive domain: estimate, [95% confidence interval], (p-value)
Basic Activity LevelApplied Activity LevelFocused Activity LevelTask OrientationCrisis ResponseInformation SeekingInformation UsageBreadth of ApproachStrategyAverage
Day 1 Green+1.35 [1.28, 1.43] (< 0.0001)1.39 [1.26, 1.52] (< 0.0001)1.44 [1.27, 1.62] (< 0.0001)1.14 [1.11, 1.17] (< 0.0001)2.35 [1.91, 2.78] (< 0.0001)1.10 [1.07, 1.14] (< 0.0001)3.94 [3.47, 4.41] (< 0.0001)1.43 [1.25, 1.60] (< 0.0001)3.77 [3.40, 4.14] (< 0.0001)1.99 [1.89, 2.09] (< 0.0001)
Day 2 Moderate CO21.20 [1.13, 1.27] (< 0.0001)1.08 [0.95, 1.21] (0.23)1.68 [1.51, 1.85] (< 0.0001)1.05 [1.02, 1.08] (0.0009)2.05 [1.63, 2.48] (< 0.0001)1.11 [1.08, 1.15] (< 0.0001)2.61 [2.15, 3.07] (< 0.0001)1.29 [1.12, 1.46] (0.0013)3.17 [2.81, 3.53] (< 0.0001)1.69 [1.59, 1.79] (< 0.0001)
Day 3 High CO20.91 [0.84, 0.98] (0.015)0.88 [0.75, 1.01] (0.081)0.85 [0.68, 1.02] (0.087)1.00 [0.97, 1.03] (0.76)1.33 [0.90, 1.75] (0.14)1.08 [1.05, 1.12] (< 0.0001)1.01 [0.55, 1.48] (0.95)0.98 [0.81, 1.15] (0.78)0.83 [0.47, 1.19] (0.36)0.99 [0.89, 1.09] (0.78)
Day 4 Green1.14 [1.06, 1.21] (0.0003)1.04 [0.91, 1.18] (0.51)1.51 [1.34, 1.68] (< 0.0001)1.03 [1.00, 1.06] (0.065)1.97 [1.54, 2.40] (< 0.0001)1.09 [1.05, 1.12] (< 0.0001)2.72 [2.26, 3.19] (< 0.0001)1.21 [1.04, 1.38] (0.018)2.83 [2.46, 3.19] (< 0.0001)1.61 [1.51, 1.71] (< 0.0001)
Day 5 Conventional (Reference)1.001.001.001.001.001.001.001.001.001.00
Day 6 Green+1.37 [1.30, 1.44] (< 0.0001)1.33 [1.20, 1.46] (< 0.0001)1.52 [1.35, 1.69] (< 0.0001)1.15 [1.12, 1.19] (< 0.0001)2.27 [1.85, 2.69] (< 0.0001)1.11 [1.08, 1.15] (< 0.0001)4.04 [3.58, 4.51] (< 0.0001)1.50 [1.33, 1.67] (< 0.0001)3.98 [3.62, 4.34] (< 0.0001)2.03 [1.93, 2.13] (< 0.0001)
R20.340.170.330.030.280.060.690.270.790.81
IEQ, indoor environmental quality.
Figure 1 Average cognitive function scores and standard error bars by domain for the Conventional, Green, and two Green+ conditions, normalized to the Green condition by dividing all scores by the average score during the Green condition.
Figure 2 Cognitive function scores by domain and participant and the corresponding carbon dioxide concentration in their cubicles. Each line represents the change in an individual’s CO2 exposure and cognitive scores from one condition to the next, normalized to the average CO2 exposure across all participants during the Green+ conditions.

Results

Green Building and Cognitive Function

The TVOC levels were constant at < 50 μg/m3 on all study days except the Conventional building day, when levels increased to 506–666 μg/m3 depending on the room. The compounds that increased in concentration included but were not limited to formaldehyde, benzaldehyde, acetaldehyde, heptane, and 2-propanol. Heptane and 2-propanol had the largest increases of the sampled compounds (Table 3). Total aldehyde concentrations were primarily driven by o-phthalaldehyde and remained relatively constant on all study days.
Cognitive function scores were higher under the Green building condition than under the Conventional building condition for all nine functional domains (Figure 1). On average, cognitive scores were 61% higher on the Green building day and 101% higher on the two Green+ building days than on the Conventional building day. The largest effects were seen for Crisis Response, Information Usage, and Strategy, all of which are indicators of higher-level cognitive function and decision making (Streufert and Swezey 1986). For Crisis Response, scores were 97% higher during the Green condition than during the Conventional condition, and 131% higher during the Green+ condition than during the Conventional condition. For Information Usage, scores obtained under the Green and Green+ conditions were 172% and 299% higher than under the Conventional condition, respectively. Finally, for Strategy, which tested the participants’ ability to plan, prioritize, and sequence actions, the Green and Green+ day scores were 183% and 288% higher than on the Conventional day, respectively (Table 5).
The raw cognitive scores for each domain were normalized to the Conventional condition and modeled by study day, controlling for participant (Table 5). The repeat simulation of the Green+ day (Day 6), which was added to the study as a quality control measure, showed similar cognitive function scores: p-values for the null hypothesis of no difference between the 2 days ranged from 0.27 for Strategy (normalized scores of 3.77 and 3.98, respectively) to 0.73 for Crisis Response (normalized scores of 2.35 and 2.27). Under the Green+ condition, participants had statistically significantly higher cognitive function scores than under the Conventional condition in all domains (p < 0.0001). Under the Green condition, particpants had higher scores than under the Conventional condition in all domains, five of which were statistically significant.
Participants scored higher on the Green+ days than on the Green day in eight of nine domains, resulting in a 25% increase in scores on average when outdoor air ventilation rates were increased. Cognitive scores were 20% higher on the Green+ days than on the moderate CO2 day when CO2 levels were higher (p-value < 0.0001), and 5% higher on the moderate CO2 day than on the Green day when outdoor air ventilation was reduced (p-value = 0.12). These estimates and p-values were produced by rerunning the “average” model in Table 5 with the Green condition as the reference category (data not shown).
The model of the average scores in Table 5 had a high R 2 value of 0.81, indicating that a significant amount of the variability in cognitive scores can be explained by these indoor-environment test conditions, leaving only 19% of the variability to be explained by all other potential intrapersonal drivers of cognitive function such as diet, the previous night’s sleep quality, and mood. For the specific domains of cognitive function, the R 2 ranged from 0.03 to 0.79.

Carbon Dioxide and Cognitive Function

The effects of CO2 on cognitive function scores while all other parameters were held constant are depicted in Figure 2. Because the air in each room was not completely mixed, there was some variability in CO2 levels between cubicles. Each line represents the change in an individual’s CO2 exposure and cognitive scores from one condition to the next, normalized to the average CO2 exposure across all participants during the Green+ conditions. For seven of the nine cognitive function domains, average cognitive scores decreased at each higher level of CO2 (Table 5). Cognitive function scores were 15% lower for the moderate CO2 day (~ 945 ppm) and 50% lower on the day with CO2 concentrations of ~1,400 ppm than on the two Green+ days (Table 5, dividing the average Green+ estimate by the moderate CO2 and high CO2 estimates, respectively). The exposure–response curve between CO2 and cognitive function is approximately linear across the CO2 concentrations used in this study; however, whether the largest difference in scores is between the Green+ conditions and the moderate CO2 condition or the moderate CO2 condition and the high CO2 condition depends on the domain (Figure 2).
Ventilation rate, CO2, and TVOCs were modeled separately from study day to capture the independent effects of each factor on cognitive function scores, averaged across all domains. A statistically significant increase in scores was associated with ventilation rate, CO2, and TVOCs (p < 0.0001 for all three parameters). On average, a 400-ppm increase in CO2 was associated with a 21% decrease in a typical participant’s cognitive scores across all domains after adjusting for participant (data not shown), a 20-cfm increase in outdoor air per person was associated with an 18% increase in these scores, and a 500-μg/m3 increase in TVOCs was associated with a 13% decrease in these scores. Although other environmental variables were not experimentally modified, some did vary over the course of the study (Table 2). There was a high degree of consistency in IEQ between the two rooms; however, ozone was significantly higher in one of the chambers on the Green day. Cognitive scores were 4% higher in the room with high ozone on this day, after accounting for baseline cognitive performance in the two rooms. These IEQ parameters were added to the model with the experimentally controlled variables and were not found to be significantly associated with cognitive function at the 0.05 significance level.

Discussion

Green Buildings and Health

We found that when participants spent a full day in a Green building, there was a significant increase in their cognitive function scores compared with when they spent a day in an environment that had been designed to simulate a conventional building by elevating VOC concentrations. The study was designed to represent conditions typically observed in many buildings; we did not include extreme exposures or choose uncommon VOC sources. Further, we selected our target levels of VOCs, ventilation rates and CO2 to be above and below the standards in LEED®, ASHRAE, and the U.S. EPA BASE study in order to evaluate how these common standards and guidelines perform (USGBC 2014, ASHRAE 2013a, U.S. EPA 1998). Our findings indicate that there may be benefits to meeting the LEED® VOC guideline of 500 μg/m3 and enhancing ventilation rates beyond the minimum requirement under ASHRAE.
The “Conventional” building simulation parameters in our study were based on conditions described in the U.S. EPA BASE study, which plausibly represent the upper end of performance for “typical” buildings in the United States in the 1990s because the owners were willing to participate in the study, introducing potential self-selection bias, and larger, “non-problem” buildings were preferentially recruited (Persily and Gorfain 2004). Therefore, the extent to which BASE buildings represent typical conventional buildings is unknown. Our findings show impacts above the 95th percentile of CO2 (945 ppm) and the mean VOC concentration in the BASE study (450 μg/m3); however, a larger proportion of the buildings in the BASE study would likely have exceeded these targets if “problem” buildings had been included in the recruitment process.
The VOC levels on the Conventional and Green/Green+ days straddled both the LEED® TVOC guidance concentration of 500 μg/m3 and the BASE mean concentration of 450 μg/m3. The common VOC sources that were added to the rooms during the Conventional building day led to increases in a range of VOCs. Previous testing with the SMS tool showed that 2 hr of painting, which exposed participants to VOCs, was associated with reductions in three of the five domains investigated (Satish et al. 2013). The lower TVOC concentrations (yet larger number of sources) in the present study were associated with statistically significant decrements in decision-making performance in five of the nine domains.

Carbon Dioxide and Ventilation

Carbon dioxide concentration in indoor environments has long been used as an indicator of ventilation and as a proxy for indoor air quality (ASHRAE 2013b). However, this conventional thinking is being challenged as the evidence mounts for CO2 as a direct pollutant, not just a marker for other pollutants (Satish et al. 2012). We found statistically significant declines in cognitive function scores when CO2 concentrations were increased to levels that are common in indoor spaces (approximately 950 ppm). In fact, this level of CO2 is considered acceptable because it would satisfy ASHRAE’s ventilation rate guidance for acceptable indoor air quality. Larger differences were seen when CO2 was raised to 1,400 ppm.
Satish et al. used the SMS tool to test the effects of CO2 exposures on the cognitive function of 22 participants, using a controlled chamber and injection of ultra-pure CO2 (Satish et al. 2012). The authors reported effects on seven of nine cognitive function domains with increasing CO2 concentration. The SMS tool was also used to test the relationship between ventilation rate and cognitive function among 16 participants (Maddalena et al. 2015). Participants scored significantly lower on eight of nine domains at low ventilation rates (12.5 cfm of outdoor air/person). In contrast to the present study, these other studies had a) a single experimental parameter; b) half-day or shorter exposures; c) multiple experimental conditions per day; d) atypical exposure targets (2,500 ppm of CO2 and 12.5 cfm outdoor air/person); and e) primarily students and college-age adults. Despite these differences, our study found similar changes in cognitive scores from a unit change in CO2 or outdoor air ventilation. Associations were consistent a) in all three study populations, indicating that knowledge workers and students were equally affected by CO2 and outdoor air ventilation, and b) at different exposure durations, indicating that even short exposures are associated with cognitive function. Given the similarities in findings, there may not be a desensitization or compensatory response from prolonged exposure. More research is necessary to investigate the presence of these responses or the lack thereof.
The CO2 exposure levels used in this study are comparable to those in a variety of indoor locations. Assessment of public housing units in Boston found median CO2 levels to be 809 ppm in conventional apartments and 1,204 ppm in the newly constructed LEED® platinum apartments (Colton et al. 2014). Corsi et al. (2002) reported CO2 concentrations > 1,000 ppm in 66% of 120 classrooms in Texas, and Shendell et al. (2004) measured CO2 concentrations > 1,000 ppm in 45% of 435 classrooms in Washington and Idaho and reported that elevated CO2 concentrations were associated with increases in student absences.

Strengths and Limitations

The study design has several notable strengths. These strengths include repeated measures of cognitive function on the same individual for control of between-subject variability, characterization of the TIEQ Lab for potential environmental confounders, repeated testing of the same condition 9 days apart on different days of the week, mid-week testing to avoid potential Monday/Friday bias, participants and cognitive function analysts blinded to test conditions, and the use of an objective measure of cognitive function.
The SMS tool is an objective assessment tool, unlike self-reported metrics, and thus is less susceptible to the participant’s environmental perceptions. Extensive work has been dedicated to testing the validity of the SMS software; correlations between scores on these tests and other measures of productivity such as income at age and job level at age exceed 0.6 (Streufert et al. 1988). The correlations are stronger for the more strategic domains, such as strategy, information usage, and crisis response, than for domains pertaining to activity, such as information search and activity level. The domains that were most affected by the exposures in this study are the same domains that are most closely related to other measures of productivity (Streufert et al. 1988). Lastly, the concordance of the scores for the two Green+ conditions suggests that a) the study was internally valid, b) there were no learning effects associated with the test, and c) day of the week (Tuesday vs. Thursday) was not a potential confounding variable.
The potential for confounding or effect modification by parameters measured or otherwise was reduced by the use of the controlled environment and by repeated measures on each participant. By testing on subsequent days, it is possible that effects from one condition were reflected in the scores obtained on the next day. The environmental factors that were not experimentally modified exhibited some variability owing to changes in outdoor conditions and participant behavior. In particular, ozone levels fluctuated significantly between some IEQ conditions (Table 2). Environmental factors other than outdoor air ventilation, CO2, and VOCs were not statistically significant predictors of cognitive scores, but the possibility of uncontrolled confounding by these factors cannot be excluded. The environmental conditions on each of the study days met design criteria. On one day (Day 4), CO2 levels were lower in the morning than in the afternoon, which influenced the reported mean concentration. The CO2 levels on this day were similar to the moderate CO2 and Conventional conditions (Day 5) during the time leading up to and during the cognitive test (926 ppm from 1400 to 1700 hours). This study used a controlled environment to individually control certain contaminants. Assessments performed in actual office environments are important to confirm the findings in a noncontrolled setting.

Conclusion

Office workers had significantly improved cognitive function scores when working in Green and Green+ environments compared with scores obtained when working in a Conventional environment. Exposure to CO2 and VOCs at levels found in conventional office buildings was associated with lower cognitive scores than those associated with levels of these compounds found in a Green building. Using low-emitting materials, which is common practice in Green buildings, reduces in-office VOC exposures. Increasing the supply of outdoor air lowers exposures to not only CO2 and VOCs but also to other indoor contaminants. Green building design that optimizes employee productivity and energy usage will require adopting energy-efficient systems and informed operating practices to maximize benefits to human health while minimizing energy consumption. This study was designed to reflect indoor office environments in which large numbers of people work every day. These exposures should be investigated in other indoor environments, such as homes, schools, and airplanes, where decrements in cognitive function and decision making could have significant impacts on productivity, learning, and safety.
Editor’s Note: In the Information Seeking column in Table 5, the p-values for Day 2 (Moderate CO2), Day 3 (High CO2), and Day 4 (Green) have been changed to < 0.0001 from 0.61, 0.35, and 0.45, respectively. In the Information Usage column in Table 5, the p-value for Day 3 (High CO2) has been changed from < 0.0001 to 0.95. The previous p-values were from a different reference category that was subsequently changed during the peer-review process. The new p-values are consistent with the current reference category, and the conclusions of the manuscript are unaffected by these changes.

Acknowledgments

We thank the study participants for volunteering and the reviewers of this manuscript for their insights that helped improve the manuscript.

References

ACGIH (American Conference of Governmental Industrial Hygienists). 2015. 2015 Guide to Occupational Exposure Values. Cincinnati, OH ACGIH.
Allen JG, MacIntosh DL, Saltzman LE, Baker BJ, Matheson JM, Recht JRet al. 2012. Elevated corrosion rates and hydrogen sulfide in homes with ‘Chinese Drywall.’. Sci Total Environ 426:113-119 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22525559/.
Allen JG, MacNaughton P, Laurent JG, Flanigan SS, Eitland ES, Spengler JD. 2015. Green buildings and health. Curr Environ Health Rep 2(3):250-258 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26231502/.
ASHRAE (American Society of Heating, Refrigerating, and Air-Conditioning Engineers). 2013a. ANSI/ASHRAE Standard 62.1-2013. Ventilation for Acceptable Indoor Air Quality. Atlanta, GA ASHRAE.
ASHRAE. 2013b. 2013 ASHRAE Handbook—Fundamentals. Atlanta, GA ASHRAE.
Bornehag CG, Sundell J, Hägerhed-Engman L, Sigsgaard T. 2005. Association between ventilation rates in 390 Swedish homes and allergic symptoms in children. Indoor Air 15:275-280 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15982274/.
Breuer K, Satish U. 2003. Emergency management simulations: an approach to the assessment of decision-making processes in complex dynamic crisis environments. In: From Modeling to Managing Security: A Systems Dynamics Approach (Gonzalez JJ, ed) Kristiansand S., Norway Høyskoleforlaget AS - Norwegian Academic Press 145-156.
Chan WR, Price PN, Sohn MD, Gadgil AJ. 2003. Analysis of U.S. residential air leakage database. Lawrence Berkeley National Labs Report Number 53367. Available: http://www.osti.gov/scitech/servlets/purl/816784 [accessed 15 March 2016].
Colton M, MacNaughton P, Vallarino J, Kane J, Bennett-Fripp M, Spengler Jet al. 2014. Indoor air quality in green vs conventional multifamily low-income housing. Environ Sci Technol 48(14):7833-7841 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24941256/.
Corsi RL, Torres VM, Sanders M, Kinney KA. 2002. Carbon dioxide levels and dynamics in elementary schools: results from the TESIAS study. In: Indoor Air 2002, 9th International Conference on Indoor Air Quality and Climate, Monterey, California (Levin H, ed) Monterey, CA Indoor Air 2002 Inc. 74-79.
Fisk WJ, Rosenfeld AH. 1997. Estimates of improved productivity and health from better indoor environments. Indoor Air 7(3):158-172. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0668.1997.t01-1-00002.x.
Hedge A. 2009. Indoor environmental quality, health and productivity. Environ Res J 4 (1/2).
Hedge A, Gaygen DE. 2010. Indoor environment conditions and computer work in an office. HVAC&R Res 16(2):123-138.
Hodgson AT, Rudd AF, Beal D, Chandra S. 2000. Volatile organic compound concentrations and emission rates in new manufactured and site-built houses. Indoor Air 10:178-192 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10979199/.
Jacobs DE, Ahonen E, Dixon SL, Dorevitch S, Breysse J, Smith Jet al. 2015. Moving into green healthy housing. J Public Health Manag Pract 21(4):345-354. https://doi.org/10.1097/PHH.0000000000000047 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24378632/.
Maddalena R, Mendell MJ, Eliseeva K, Chan WR, Sullivan DP, Russell Met al. 2015. Effects of ventilation rate per person and per floor area on perceived air quality, sick building syndrome symptoms, and decision-making. Indoor Air 25(4):362-370. https://doi.org/10.1111/ina.12149 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25142723/.
Mendell MJ. 1993. Non-specific symptoms in office workers: a review and summary of the epidemiologic literature. Indoor Air 3(4):227-236. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0668.1993.00003.x.
Nishihara N, Wargocki P, Tanabe S. 2014. Cerebral blood flow, fatigue, mental effort, and task performance in offices with two different pollution loads. Build Environ 71:153-164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2013.09.018.
Noris F, Adamkiewicz G, Delp WW, Hotchi T, Russell M, Singer BCet al. 2013. Indoor environmental quality benefits of apartment energy retrofits. Build Environ 68:170-178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2013.07.003.
NRC (National Research Council). 2007. Green Schools: Attributes for Health and Learning. Washington, DC National Acadamies Press.
Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA). 1991. Nitrogen dioxide in workplace atmospheres (ion chromatography). Available: https://www.osha.gov/dts/sltc/methods/inorganic/id182/id182.pdf [accessed 5 April 2016].
Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA). 2008. Ozone in workplace atmospheres (impregnated glass fiber filter). Available: https://www.osha.gov/dts/sltc/methods/inorganic/id214/id214.pdf [accessed 5 April 2016].
Persily A. 2015. Challenges in developing ventilation and indoor air quality standards: the story of ASHRAE Standard 62. Build Environ 91:61-69.
Persily AK, Gorfain J. 2004. Analysis of Ventilation Data from the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency Building Assessment Survey and Evaluation (BASE) Study, NISTIR 7145. Bethesda. MD National Institute of Standards and Technology, U.S. Department of Commerce.
R Core Team. 2015. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria R Foundation for Statistical Computing Available: http://www.R-project.org [accessed 15 March 2015].
Riesenberg DE, Arehart-Treichel J. 1986. “Sick building” syndrome plagues workers, dwellers. JAMA 255:3063. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.1986.03370220021005 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3702012/.
Satish U, Cleckner L, Vasselli J. 2013. Impact of VOCs on decision making and productivity. Intell Build Int 5(4):213-220. https://doi.org/10.1080/17508975.2013.812956.
Satish U, Mendell MJ, Shekhar K, Hotchi T, Sullivan D, Streufert Set al. 2012. Is CO2 an indoor pollutant? Direct effects of Low-to-moderate CO2 concentrations on human decision-making performance. Environ Health Perspect 120:1671-1677. https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.1104789 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23008272/.
Satish U, Streufert S, Dewan M, Voort SV. 2004. Improvements in simulated real-world relevant performance for patients with seasonal allergic rhinitis: impact of desloratadine. Allergy 59(4):415-420 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15005765/.
Shendell DG, Prill R, Fisk WJ, Apte MG, Blake D, Faulkner D. 2004. Associations between classroom CO2 concentrations and student attendance in Washington and Idaho. Indoor Air 14:333-341 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15330793/.
Singh A, Syal M, Grady SC, Korkmaz S. 2010. Effects of green buildings on employee health and productivity. Am J Public Health 100(9):1665-1668. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009.180687 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20634460/.
Streufert S, Pogash R, Piasecki M. 1988. Simulation-based assessment of managerial competence: reliability and validity. Pers Psychol 41(3):537-557. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1988.tb00643.x.
Streufert S, Swezey RW. 1986. Complexity, Managers, and Organizations. Orlando, FL Academic Press.
Swezey RW, Streufert S, Satish U, Siem FM. 1998. Preliminary development of a computer-based team performance assessment simulation. Int J Cogn Ergonomics 2:163-179.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA). 1999. Compendium method TO-11A: Determination of formaldehyde in ambient air using absorbent cartridge followed by high performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) [Active sampling methodology]. Available: https://www3.epa.gov/ttnamti1/files/ambient/airtox/to-11ar.pdf [accessed 5 April 2016].
U.S. EPA (U.S. Environmental Protection Agency). 1999. Compendium method TO-15: Determination of volatile organic compounds (VOCs) in air collected in specially-prepared canisters and analyzed by gas chromatography/mass spectrometry (GC/MS). Available: https://www3.epa.gov/ttnamti1/files/ambient/airtox/to-15r.pdf [accessed 5 April 2016].
U.S. EPA (U.S. Environmental Protection Agency). 1998. Building Assessment Survey and Evaluation Study. Available: https://www.epa.gov/indoor-air-quality-iaq/building-assessment-survey-and-evaluation-study [accessed 22 January 2015].
USGBC (U.S. Green Building Council). 2014. LEED v4 User Guide. Washington DC USGBC Available: http://www.usgbc.org/resources/leed-v4-user-guide [accessed 15 April 2015].
Wargocki P, Wyon D, Sundell J, Clausen G, Fanger PO. 2000. The effects of outdoor air supply rate in an office on perceived air quality, sick building syndrome (SBS) symptoms and productivity. Indoor Air 10(4)), 222-236 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11089327/.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Environmental Health Perspectives
Volume 124Issue 6June 2016
Pages: 805 - 812
PubMed: 26502459

History

Received: 4 April 2015
Accepted: 12 October 2015
Published online: 26 October 2015

Authors

Affiliations

Joseph G. Allen [email protected]
Exposure, Epidemiology, and Risk Program, Department of Environmental Health, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
Piers MacNaughton
Exposure, Epidemiology, and Risk Program, Department of Environmental Health, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
Usha Satish
Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, SUNY-Upstate Medical School, Syracuse, New York, USA
Suresh Santanam
Industrial Assessment Center, Center of Excellence, Syracuse University, Syracuse, New York, USA
Jose Vallarino
Exposure, Epidemiology, and Risk Program, Department of Environmental Health, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
John D. Spengler
Exposure, Epidemiology, and Risk Program, Department of Environmental Health, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts, USA

Notes

Address correspondence to J.G. Allen, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, 401 Park Dr., Landmark Center, 404-L, Boston, MA 02215 USA. Telephone: (617) 384-8475. E-mail: [email protected]

Competing Interests

United Technologies was not involved in the data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, data presentation, or drafting of the manuscript.

Competing Interests

The authors declare they have no actual or potential competing financial interests.

Funding Information

This research was supported by a gift from United Technologies to the Center for Health and the Global Environment at the Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health. J.G.A.’s time was primarily supported by faculty startup funds, J.D.S.’s time was primarily funded by his endowed chair, and P.M.’s time was supported by National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (NIEHS) environmental epidemiology training grant 5T32ES007069-35. United Technologies Research Center provided limited input during the study design phase (support for adding a control day and adding a third CO2 test level).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

About Article Metrics


Citations

Download citation

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click DOWNLOAD.

Cited by

  • Designing LTC Physical Work Environments to Support Worker Well-being: A Review and Recommendations, Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 10.1016/j.jamda.2024.105326, 26, 1, (105326), (2025).
  • Machine learning based multi-objective optimisation of energy consumption, thermal comfort and CO2 concentration in energy-efficient naturally ventilated residential dwellings, Building and Environment, 10.1016/j.buildenv.2024.112255, 267, (112255), (2025).
  • A novel dataset of indoor environmental conditions in work-from-home settings, Building and Environment, 10.1016/j.buildenv.2024.112222, 267, (112222), (2025).
  • Indoor environment and brain health across the life course: A systematic review, Building and Environment, 10.1016/j.buildenv.2024.112156, 267, (112156), (2025).
  • Chemistry of supercritical CO2 processing, Chemistry of Thermal and Non-Thermal Food Processing Technologies, 10.1016/B978-0-443-22182-8.00015-2, (313-351), (2025).
  • The Impact of Green Building Design on Indoor Air Quality and Occupant Experience, African Journal of Environmental Sciences and Renewable Energy, 10.62154/ajesre.2024.016.010403, 16, 1, (203-215), (2024).
  • Retirement Village Design Features: Generation X’s Expectation, Pertanika Journal of Social Sciences and Humanities, 10.47836/pjssh.32.2.15, 32, 2, (675-701), (2024).
  • The Effects of New COVID-19 Classrooms Construction Project Towards Students’ Academic Performance in Public Secondary Schools in Lushoto District Council, Tanzania, International Journal of Innovative Science and Research Technology (IJISRT), 10.38124/ijisrt/IJISRT24AUG1604, (2882-2887), (2024).
  • A Systematic Review of the Vertical Green System for Balancing Ecology and Urbanity, Water, 10.3390/w16111472, 16, 11, (1472), (2024).
  • Peplospheric Influences on Local Greenhouse Gas and Aerosol Variability at the Lamezia Terme WMO/GAW Regional Station in Calabria, Southern Italy: A Multiparameter Investigation, Sustainability, 10.3390/su162310175, 16, 23, (10175), (2024).
  • See more

View Options

View options

PDF

View PDF

Restore your content access

Enter your email address to restore your content access:

Note: This functionality works only for purchases done as a guest. If you already have an account, log in to access the content to which you are entitled.

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Copy the content Link

Share on social media