Skip to main content

Abstract

Background:

Herbicides are the most used class of pesticides worldwide, and insect repellents are widely used globally. Yet, there is a dearth of studies characterizing the associations between these chemical groups and human neurobehavior. Experimental studies suggest that glyphosate and 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid (2,4-D) herbicides can affect neurobehavior and the cholinergic and glutamatergic pathways in the brain. We aim to assess whether herbicides and insect repellents are associated with neurobehavioral performance in adolescents.

Methods:

We assessed 519 participants (11–17 years of age) living in agricultural communities in Ecuador. We quantified urinary concentrations of glyphosate, 2,4-D, and two N,N-diethyl-meta-toluamide (DEET) insect repellent metabolites [3-(diethylcarbamoyl)benzoic acid (DCBA) and 3-(ethylcarbamoyl)benzoic acid (ECBA)] using isotope-dilution mass spectrometry. We assessed neurobehavioral performance using 9 subtests across 5 domains (attention/inhibitory control, memory/learning, language, visuospatial processing, and social perception). We characterized the associations using generalized estimating equations and multiple imputation for metabolites below detection limits. Models were adjusted for demographic and anthropometric characteristics, urinary creatinine, and sexual maturation. Mediation by salivary cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone, 17β-estradiol, and testosterone was assessed using structural equation modeling.

Results:

The mean of each neurobehavioral domain score was between 7.0 and 8.7 [standard deviation (SD) range: 2.0–2.3]. Glyphosate was detected in 98.3% of participants, 2,4-D in 66.2%, DCBA in 63.3%, and ECBA in 33.4%. 2,4-D was negatively associated with all neurobehavioral domains, but statistically significant associations were observed with attention/inhibition [score difference per 50% higher metabolite concentration (β)=0.19 95% confidence interval (CI): 0.31, 0.07], language [β=0.12 (95% CI: 0.23, 0.01)], and memory/learning [β=0.11 (95% CI: 0.22, 0.01)]. Glyphosate had a statistically significant negative association only with social perception [β=0.08 (95% CI: 0.14, 0.01)]. DEET metabolites were not associated with neurobehavioral performance. Mediation by gender and adrenal hormones was not observed.

Conclusion:

This study describes worse neurobehavioral performance associated with herbicide exposures in adolescents, particularly with 2,4-D. Replication of these findings among other pediatric and adult populations is needed. https://doi.org/10.1289/EHP11383

Introduction

Following the introduction of N-(phosphonomethyl) glycine (glyphosate)-resistant “Roundup ready” crops in 1996 and 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid (2,4-D)-resistant crops in 2014, there has been a global 15-fold increase in glyphosate use and a substantial increase in 2,4-D use, making them the most widely used herbicides in the world.1,2 2,4-D is a broadleaf herbicide, which mimics auxin and kills dicotyledon (dicots) without affecting monocotyledon (monocots),3 allowing agricultural users to selectively target weeds. Glyphosate is a nonselective, broad-spectrum herbicide that targets the enzyme 5-enolpyruvyl-3-shikimate phosphate synthase and is slow acting.46 It is generally used in agriculture to control vegetation by damaging growth or by acting as a desiccant.3,5 Insect repellants, like N,N-diethyl-meta-toluamide (DEET), are also widely used, as an estimated 2.6 to 4.5 million pounds of DEET are produced or imported annually in countries like the United States.7 In a random sample of 40 homes in the Ecuadorian coastal region, 32% of homes used DEET-based repellents for vector control.8 There is a considerable use of herbicides and insect repellants in South America due to the growing agricultural and floricultural industries,9 which can result in increased exposure to these chemicals in both occupational and nonoccupational populations.
In mice models, subchronic and chronic exposure to glyphosate-based herbicides from in utero to adulthood resulted in neurobehavioral changes, such as decreased locomotor activity, impaired recognition memory, cognitive function alterations, and increased levels of anxiety and depression.1013 Adult zebrafish exposed to chronic, low environmentally relevant concentrations of glyphosate over 2 weeks showed increased anxiety and increased levels of lipid peroxidation, a marker of oxidative stress.14 Doses of 2,4-D or 2,4-D esters have also been linked to behavioral changes and neurological toxicity (depressed locomotor activity, circling behavior, increased limb grip strength, increased landing foot splay) along with altered serotonin and dopamine levels in rats.1518 Furthermore, when zebrafish larvae are exposed to minimal doses of 2,4-D, it leads to changes in their swimming behaviors.19 As for DEET, rats given a single oral dose of 500mg/kg, a toxic level of exposure, developed increased thermal response time and increased exploratory behavior.20 These neurobehavioral changes did not occur at lower exposures, even with consistent exposure across a 14-d period.20 Additionally, dermal application of 400mg/kg of DEET, a concentration comparable to a human exposure dose, on adult male rats led to reduced neurobehavioral performance of sensorimotor functions, such as beam-walking score and grip response assessments.21,22 Worse performance was observed when coupled with permethrin or malathion exposure.21,22
Despite compelling data in animal models, epidemiological evidence in humans that correlates 2,4-D, glyphosate, or DEET exposure with cognitive effects is sparse. For glyphosate, a cross-sectional study of 288 Ugandan farmers identified a positive association between exposure, assessed via pesticide-specific yearly exposure-intensity scores based on self-reported glyphosate use, and impaired visual memory [0.103; 95% Bayesian credible interval (BCI): 0.24, 0]; units decrease in Benton visual retention test scores per interquartile range increase in annual glyphosate exposure.23 Prenatal exposure to glyphosate, assessed by calculating pounds per acre applied within a 2,000-m radius of mothers’ homes using data from the California Pesticide Use Reporting (CA-PUR) program, was associated with elevated risk of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [odds ratio (OR)=1.16; 95% confidence interval (CI): 1.06, 1.27] and ASD with intellectual disability [OR=1.33 (95% CI: 1.05, 1.69)].24,25 For 2,4-D, no associations were reported between urinary metabolites of 2,4-D and neurobehavioral performance in a representative sample of adults in the United States.26 Per unit increase in log based 10 concentrations of 2,4-D, there was no statistically significant changes for simple reaction time (β=3.86, p=0.62), symbol digit substitution mean total latency (β=0.93, p=0.44), and number of errors (β=0.0007, p=0.99), or the serial digit learning trials to criterion (β=0.39, p=0.19) and total score (β=1.03, p=0.10). However, in a study of 232 infants from rural Southeast China, participants who had umbilical cord blood plasma concentrations of 2,4-D over 1.17 ng/mL had deficits in auditory processing measured by slower wave V latencies [0.12 milliseconds (95% CI: 0.03, 0.22)] and auditory brainstem response central conduction time [0.15 milliseconds (95% CI: 0.05, 0.25)] compared to infants with nondetectable concentrations.27 Another study found that workers of a 2,4-D plant had decreased nerve conduction velocity compared to manufacturing plant workers that had no direct contact with 2,4-D (mean=34.0 vs. 40.1m/s, p<0.02).28 While nerve conduction velocity may not correlate with central nervous system (CNS) development, these findings highlight the neurotoxic potential of 2,4-D exposures. DEET exposure has been associated with neurological changes, including impaired cognitive functioning, agitation, and aggressive behavior in humans.29 More severe neurological symptoms related to DEET exposure, such as seizures and encephalopathy, have been observed in children who experienced high exposures according to clinical reports and Poison Control Center records.30,31 Currently, data on the effects of herbicide and DEET exposure on neurobehavioral or cognitive performance among adolescents are lacking.
Herbicide and insect repellent exposures could potentially lead to neurobehavioral and cognitive changes through multiple mechanisms. Glyphosate exposure may lead to membrane depolarization by increasing entry of sodium and calcium ions from the extracellular medium through overstimulation of N-methyl-D-aspartate glutamatergic receptors (NMDAR), inhibition of acetylcholinesterase (AChE), or increased expression of the Wnt-5a mRNA.32 Glyphosate also increases intracellular reactive oxygen species concentrations that are not sufficiently counteracted by endogenous antioxidants.32 Animal model studies have shown that 2,4-D exposure during sensitive periods of development may lead to lower myeline deposition and numbers.27 Proper cortical myelination has been found to be important to cognitive development and brain plasticity.33,34 In insects, DEET affects odorant receptors as well as olfactory and gustatory receptor neurons and can impact the peripheral nervous system by inducing neuroexcitation and toxicity and reversibly inhibiting AChE in insects and mammals.31,3538 DEET’s effects may lead to adverse neurologic effects in humans through a similar mechanism.31
The endocrine system is crucial to the proper development of the CNS and human behavior.3941 Considering that glyphosate and 2,4-D have endocrine disrupting characteristics,39,4250 endocrine changes induced by 2,4-D or glyphosate may mediate neurobehavioral alterations associated with those exposures.39,42 There has been no evidence of endocrine alterations in humans due to DEET. In animal studies, intermediate and chronic oral exposure to DEET did not result in alterations of the adrenal, pituitary, thyroid, or parathyroid glands.29 Our prior work with adolescents in agricultural communities suggested that sex and adrenal hormones accounted for a portion of the effect modification seen by sex on the association between AChE activity, a marker of cholinesterase inhibitor pesticide exposure (i.e., organophosphate and carbamate insecticides), and mood symptoms in adolescents.51 In the same cohort, elevated testosterone [OR=1.78 (95% CI: 0.98, 3.23)], cortisol [OR=1.69 (95% CI: 0.95, 2.99)], and estradiol [OR=2.43 (95% CI: 1.01, 53.84)] concentrations were associated with altered anxiety, while elevated estradiol concentration [OR=4.75 (95% CI: 1.95, 11.56)] was associated with depression symptoms.52
The aim of our study is to characterize the associations of urinary concentrations of glyphosate, 2,4-D, and metabolites of DEET [3-(diethylcarbamoyl) benzoic acid (DCBA) and 3-(ethylcarbamoyl) benzoic acid (ECBA)] with neurobehavioral performance in Ecuadorian adolescents and whether these associations are mediated by gonadal and adrenal hormones.

Methods

Participants

Established in 2008, the Secondary Exposures to Pesticides among Children and Adolescents (ESPINA) study is a prospective cohort study that examines the associations of subclinical pesticide exposures on human development. In 2008, 313 boys and girls 4–9 years of age living in Pedro Moncayo County, Pichincha province, Ecuador, were examined. Most participants (n=228, 73%) were recruited using data from the 2004 Survey of Access and Demand of Health Services (SADHS), a representative survey of the County conducted by collaboration of Fundación Cimas del Ecuador, the Local Rural Governments of Pedro Moncayo, and community members. The remaining participants (n=85, 27%) were enrolled through community announcements given by governing councils, leaders, and by word-of-mouth. Recruitment aimed to achieve a balanced distribution of participants who lived with a flower plantation worker and participants who did not live with any agricultural workers. Further details of the 2008 examination have been published.53
A participant flow chart can be found in Figure 1. In 2016, we conducted two follow-up examinations of participants 11–17 years of age: a) April, which included 330 participants (11–17 years of age) and b) July to October, which included 535 participants (comprising 238 participants examined in 2008, 330 examined in April 2016, and 316 new volunteers). As in 2008, new participants were recruited using the System of Local and Community Information (SILC), a large geospatial database that contains information of the 2016 Pedro Moncayo County Community Survey (formerly the SADHS). This community health survey was administered in-person at participants’ homes and captures data from 50% of the population living in Pedro Moncayo County. The present analysis included participants examined in the July to October examination, of whom we included 519 who had information for all covariates of interest (528 participants had urinary pesticide metabolite measures, and 9 had missing covariate information).
Figure 1. Participant flow chart with sample sizes between 2008 and 2016 of participants included in the present analyses of the ESPINA study. n=311 of 330 participants examined in April 2016 also participated in the July to October 2016 examinations. Note: ESPINA, Secondary Exposures to Pesticides among Children and Adolescents.
We acquired informed consent for participation from parents and obtained parental permission and child assent for participating. This study was approved by the institutional review boards at the University of California San Diego (UCSD), Universidad San Francisco de Quito, and the Ministry of Public Health of Ecuador.

Setting

Pedro Moncayo County is located in the Ecuadorian Andes and houses a large floriculture industry that is vital to the County’s economy. It employs 21% of all adults,53 and its greenhouse floricultural crop land comprises 4.47% of the geographic area [1,495 hectares (ha)]. Flower crops in Pedro Moncayo are sprayed with over 50 different fungicides and over 20 different insecticides by workers using hand sprayers.54,55 Other crops in the County exposed to either herbicides, insecticides, and/or fungicides include corn, wheat, barley, potatoes, strawberries, and leafed greens. In Ecuador in 2021, there were 130 glyphosate-based herbicides and 31 2,4-D-based herbicides registered.56 In 2016, it was estimated that an average of 16.25kg of pesticides are applied per hectare of agricultural land in Ecuador demonstrating its high use in the region.57 Given the setting of Pedro Moncayo, potential pathways of pesticide exposure that may influence urinary pesticide concentrations include pesticide drift from pesticide-treated agricultural crops (flowers, wheat, corn, soybean, barley, etc.) onto populations nearby, cohabitation with an agricultural worker (para-occupational exposure), residential pesticide use, or contact with contaminated sources (i.e., dust, water, food).58 All participants at the time of the ESPINA assessment reported not working in agriculture, thus occupational exposure of pesticides is not or rarely anticipated in this population.

Examination

In 2016, children were examined in schools twice, first in April and again between July and October during the summer closure or during weekends. Of those who were examined in April, 311 participants were also examined in the July to October exam. We identified whether participants completed the attention & inhibitory domain assessment from the April 2016 examination to assess for retest learning effect (described in Statistical Analysis), but otherwise only data collected during the July to October examination was included in the analysis. Examiners were kept blind to participants’ pesticide exposure status. Interviews of participants’ caregivers collected information on demographics, monthly salary, parental education, and participant education. Children’s height was measured following recommended procedures to the nearest 1mm,59 using a height board, and weight was measured using a digital scale (Tanita model 0108 MC; Corporation of America, Arlington Heights, IL, USA). Sexual maturation rating (SMR) was assessed using Tanner Staging based on self-reported breast size and pubic hair growth/distribution for girls and pubic hair growth/distribution for boys, using modified Tanner drawings from Rasmussen et al. as a reference.6062 There was only one participant who identified as being White. Therefore, race and ethnicity were combined and dichotomized into White or Mestizo and Indigenous categories. Erythrocytic AChE activity and hemoglobin concentration were measured from a single finger stick sample using the EQM Test-mate ChE Cholinesterase test system 400 and AChE Erythrocyte Cholinesterase assay kit 470 (EQM, Cincinnati, OH, USA).53 The distance between each participant’s home to the nearest flower plantation, and the surface area of flower plantations within a 150-m buffer of the participant’s home was calculated using ArcGIS 9.3 (ESRI, Redlands, CA, USA).53

Neurobehavioral testing.

Neurobehavioral performance was measured using the NEPSY-II test (NCS Pearson, San Antonio, TX).64 During the July to October 2016 examination, trained psychologists blinded to participant exposure status tested participants in 9 subtests across five domains as follows: a) attention & inhibitory control (also known as attention and executive functioning, subtests: auditory attention & response set, inhibition); b) language (subtests: comprehension of instructions, speeded naming); c) memory & learning (subtests: immediate and delayed memory for faces); d) visuospatial processing (subtests: design copying, geometric puzzles); and e) social perception (subtest: affect recognition). Two subtests required translation into Spanish using terminology appropriate for the local population (auditory attention and response set and comprehension of instructions). The translation was approved by NCS Pearson. Participants were examined alone in a quiet room by the examiner. Attention & inhibitory control were also assessed in April 2016 among 303 participants included in the present analyses. NEPSY-II scaled scores for each subtest can range from 1 to 19, and have a mean of 10 [standard deviation (SD)=3] that is based on a U.S. normative sample.65,66
NEPSY scaled scores for each subtest were used in analyses; these values are age-standardized based on a national sample of children in the United States.66 Scaled scores for the NEPSY subtest were calculated using the NEPSY-II scoring assistant software (NCS Pearson, Inc., San Antonio, TX), and higher scores indicate better performance for all subtests. Domain scores were calculated by averaging primary scaled score from all subtests within each domain. For subtests that included either correct and error components (i.e., auditory attention and response set) or time and error components (i.e., inhibition, speeded naming, visuomotor precision), the combined scaled scores representing the combination of both components were used as primary scaled scores. Affect recognition was the only subtest in the social perception domain and, as such, the social perception domain is equivalent to the affect recognition scaled score. Additional details of subtest scoring have been published elsewhere.6668

Quantification of urinary creatinine, pesticide biomarkers, and hormones.

Urine samples used for measuring creatinine and pesticide biomarkers and saliva samples used for measuring hormones were collected on the same day for each participant during the July to October examination. Urinary concentrations of creatinine and pesticide biomarkers were measured in samples collected upon awakening. Participants brought the urine samples to the examination site in the morning where they were aliquoted and frozen at 20°C. At the end of each day, samples were transported to Quito for storage at 80°C. Samples were then transported overnight to UCSD at 20°C using a courier and stored at 80°C at UCSD. Samples were then shipped overnight at 20°C from UCSD to the National Center for Environmental Health, Division of Laboratory Sciences of the CDC (Atlanta, GA) for quantification of 2,4-D, 3,5,6-trichloro-2-pyridinol (TCPy) and para-nitrophenol (PNP), DCBA, and ECBA, and to the Laboratory for Exposure Assessment and Development in Environmental Research at Emory University (Atlanta, GA) for quantification of glyphosate and creatinine. Quality control/quality assurance protocols were followed to ensure data accuracy and reliability of the analytical measurements. All of the study samples were reextracted if quality control failed the statistical evaluation.69
Glyphosate.
Urine aliquots (250μL) were spiked with isotopically labeled glyphosate, diluted to 1mL with doubly deionized water, and extracted using a C18 solid phase extraction (SPE). Glyphosate was derivatized to create its heptafluorobutyl analogue then concentrated for analysis. To measure urinary glyphosate, all urine samples (aliquots) were randomized using a Fisher-Yates shuffling algorithm prior to analysis to reduce potential batch effects.70,71 Concentrated extracts were analyzed using gas chromatography–mass spectrometry using electron impact ionization in the multiple ion monitoring mode. The limit of detection (LOD) was 0.25μg/L with a relative standard deviation (RSD) of 3%.
DEET metabolites.
ECBA and DCBA were quantified in urine using a method described in detail elsewhere.69 In brief, the analytical method is based on enzymatic hydrolysis of 0.2mL of urine and online SPE to release, extract, and concentrate the target biomarkers, followed reverse-phase high-performance liquid chromatography–tandem mass spectrometry (HPLC-MS/MS) using electrospray ionization (ESI). The LOD for ECBA and DCBA was 0.2μg/L. The precision of the measurements, expressed as the percent RSD of multiple measures of two urine-based quality control (QC) materials, was below 6%, depending on the biomarker and concentration.
2,4-D, TCPy, and PNP.
These biomarkers were extracted from 1.0mL of urine and concentrated using a semi-automated SPE system, followed by HPLC-MS/MS as described in detail.72 The method LOD for 2,4-D, TCPy and PNP were 0.15μg/L, 0.1μg/L, and 0.1μg/L, respectively, and the precision, calculated as described above, was below 7% RSD. PNP and TCPy concentrations were only used to improve the multiple imputation estimates for biomarker concentrations that were below the LOD (see below).
Creatinine.
Urinary creatinine was quantified using HPLC-MS/MS with ESI. A 10-μL aliquot of urine was diluted prior to analysis.73 No further sample preparation was performed prior to analysis. The LOD was 5mg/dL with an RSD of 7%.
Hormones.
Salivary concentrations of estradiol, testosterone, cortisol, and dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) were measured using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays (ELISA) (Salimetrics, Carlsbad, CA) at the UCSD Integrative Health and Mind-Body Biomarker Laboratory.7477 Participants collected saliva samples through passive-drool upon awakening the same day they provided urine samples for metabolite collection. The saliva samples were stored at 80°C until assayed. Levels of cortisol, testosterone, and DHEA were measured in both girls and boys, while estradiol was only measured in boys, as the levels of estradiol in women vary according to the stage of the menstrual cycle.
Imputation for values below the LOD.
We used two imputation methods for pesticide metabolite concentrations below the LOD as follows: imputation using a constant (LOD/2) and multiple imputation. Imputation was needed for 1.7% (n=9) of samples for glyphosate, 33.8% (n=174) of samples for 2,4-D, 36.4% (n=192) of samples for DCBA, and 66.6% (n=342) of samples for ECBA. The multiple imputation method was built as a log-logistic regression model that was fitted using backward elimination selection where variables were retained if they had a significance level of p<0.10. We used the backward elimination since the forward or stepwise selection generally yields incorrect results.78 The initial model used for backwards stepwise regression included the following 15 variables defined a priori that were considered to be associated with pesticide exposure: age, gender, race, body mass index (BMI)-for-age z-score (z-BMI-for-age), height-for-age z-score (z-height-for-age), monthly family income, tanner maturation score, creatinine concentration, AChE activity, urinary concentrations of TCPy and PNP, flower crop area within 150m of the participant’s home, distance from the house to the contour of the nearest flower crop, and cohabitation with a floricultural or agricultural worker. Prediction models using multiple imputation were run 1,000 times (see Statistical Analysis). The observed pesticide biomarker concentrations (concentrations above LOD) and the multiple imputed concentrations for samples with censored data were included in the generalized estimated equation (GEE) models as separate variables. This maintained the robustness of observed variables, while allowing us to improve observations and power of the overall analysis. Given the high detectability of glyphosate, multiple imputation was not conducted for this biomarker.
Imputation for missing variables.
We imputed missing information for parental education and household income to maximize the observations included in this study. Average parental education was imputed for eight adolescents. First, we used the reported education from 2008 to impute father’s education for four participants and to impute mother’s education for five participants. For the remaining four participants with missing paternal education, and three participants with missing maternal education, we used a random imputation based on a normal distribution of paternal education and maternal education, respectively. The average parental education was then calculated from the imputed values. Household income was imputed for 11 adolescents. Five of these adolescents had household income imputed from what was reported from the 2008 examination. For the remaining 6 adolescents, income was imputed based on the average parental education (years). We calculated the cohort’s mean±SD income in 2016 for each year of average parental education. For those with missing income, we imputed the value based on a random normal distribution of the corresponding parental education. Methods used for parental education and income imputation have been previously reported.52

Statistical analysis.

This is a cross-sectional study that used data from the ESPINA July to October 2016 examination. Descriptive statistics were calculated across tertiles of the herbicide summary score (Table 1). We calculated means and SD for normally distributed variables, medians, and interquartile ranges (IQR) for nonnormally distributed variables, or column percentage for categorical variables for model covariates. All variables identified to be nonnormally distributed were natural log (ln)-transformed. We calculated the p-value for trend (p-trend) for participant characteristics across tertiles of the herbicide summary score using unadjusted linear regression for continuous variables (continuous participant characteristic=herbicide summary score) and unadjusted logistic regression for categorical variables (categorical participant characteristic=herbicide summary score). Characteristics were identified to be statistically significantly different across the herbicide summary score if it had a p<0.05. For characteristics that differed across herbicide summary score, we ran an adjusted GEE to further assess whether there were still differences. To evaluate how representative of the population of Pedro Moncayo our study sample was in terms of age, gender, and cohabitation with agricultural workers, we compared ESPINA distributions with those of the 2016 Community Health Survey described above. We calculated geometric means of creatinine adjusted concentrations for 2,4-D, glyphosate, DCBA, and ECBA concentrations (μg/g) that were imputed using a constant (LOD/2). We compared the biomarker concentrations to those that are presented in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) biomonitoring data to determine how exposure patterns differed from our cohort. 2,4-D, glyphosate, and DEET biomarkers were measured in the general population of adolescents in the United States from 2013 to 2014 or 2015 to 2016.79 NHANES biomarker concentration was measured in 428 individuals for 2,4-D (2013–2014), 309 individuals for glyphosate (2015–2016), 400 individuals for DCBA (2015–2016), and 399 individuals for ECBA (2015–2016). Methods used to measure 2,4-D,72 glyphosate,80 ECBA,69 and DCBA69 biomarkers in the NHANES study have been previously reported. Biomarker concentrations were compared to NHANES participants 12–19 years of age. In order to assess whether results are influenced by co-exposures to other pesticides, we conducted a Pearson correlation matrix of 2,4-D, glyphosate, DCBA, and ECBA with acetamiprid-N-desmethyl (AND) (neonicotinoid), PNP (organophosphate), TCPy (organophosphate), and 3-phenoxybenzoic acid (3-PBA) (pyrethroid).
Table 1 Participant characteristics of the July to October 2016 ESPINA study examination (n=519) based in Pedro Moncayo, Ecuador.
 OverallHerbicide summary scorep-TrendDetectable
Tertile 1 (0.17–0.75)Tertile 2 (0.75–1.32)Tertile 3 (1.32–9.09)
n%n%n%n%
Cohort519100.0%17333.3%17333.3%17333.3%
Gender0.02
 Female26651.3%9957.2%9052.0%7744.5%
 Male25348.7%7442.8%8348.0%9655.5%
Race0.26
 Indigenous11422.0%3319.1%3922.5%4224.3%
 Mestizo or White40578.0%14080.9%13477.5%13175.7%
Lives with an agricultural worker0.49
 No16932.6%5934.1%5431.2%5632.4%
 Yes35067.4%11465.9%11968.8%11767.6%
Examination period0.56
 July through September49294.8%16595.4%16193.1%16696.0%
 October275.2%84.6%126.9%74.0%
Completed attention & inhibitory domain in April examination0.40
 Yes30358.4%9454.3%10661.3%10359.5%
 No21641.6%7946%6738.7%7040.5%
Mother worked in agriculture while pregnant0.77
 Yes22243.1%8248.0%6940.0%7141.3%
 No28756.9%8952.0%10160.0%9758.7%
 Missing10235
Mother lived with agricultural worker while pregnant0.36
 Yes22143.4%7040.9%7141.8%8047.6%
 No28856.6%10159.1%9958.2%8852.4%
 Missing10235
 nMean±SDnMean±SDnMean±SDnMean±SD
Age (y)51914.46±1.7617314.69±1.7817314.26±1.7517314.44±1.730.14
Z-score BMI for age (SD)5190.38±0.841730.50±0.871730.35±0.841730.30±0.800.05
Z-score height for age (SD)5191.49±0.901731.51±0.831731.43±0.881731.54±0.990.99
Tanner score5192.89±0.951732.98±0.931732.80±1.031722.88±0.890.63
Crop areas within 150m of homes (m2)5192,054±4,8231732,187±4,9001731,971±4,7451732,003±4,8480.50
Acetylcholinesterase (U/mL)5193.70±0.551733.69±0.561733.69±0.541733.73±0.540.92
Hemoglobin (mg/dL)51912.95±1.1817312.93±1.3017312.96±1.0117312.96±1.200.91
 nMedian (25th–75th percentile)nMedian (25th–75th percentile)nMedian (25th–75th percentile)nMedian (25th–75th percentile)
Testosterone (pg/mL)51539.63 (26.04–67.25)17237.22 (25.24–58.38)17040.26 (24.87–62.08)17344.24 (27.58–81.23)0.02
17β-Estradiol (pg/mL)2470.43 (0.30–0.59)720.45 (0.30–0.60)810.42 (0.25–0.55)940.43 (0.32–0.59)0.42
Cortisol, μg/dL5180.21 (0.14–0.30)1730.20 (0.14–0.29)1720.18 (0.11–0.29)1730.23 (0.15–0.32)0.45
Dehydroepiandrosterone (pg/mL)49861.21 (30.24–105.91)16870.27 (34.09–112.83)16551.36 (26.92–100.60)16563.36 (34.00–103.02)0.61
Monthly income (imputed) (USD)a519$500 ($372$720)173$500 ($370$720)173$500 ($375$700)173$560 ($370$732)0.65
Distance to nearest floriculture field (m)519329 (111–659)173332 (107–645)173334 (111–611)173307 (118–685)0.08
Average parental education (imputed) (y)b5197.00 (6.00–10.50)1738.00 (6.00–10.50)1737.00 (6.00–10.50)1737.00 (6.00–9.50)0.45
Participant education level (y)5199.00 (8.00–11.00)17310.0 (9.00–11.00)1739.00 (8.00–10.00)1739.00 (8.00–10.00)0.04
 nGeo Mean (95% CI)nGeo Mean (95% CI)nGeo Mean (95% CI)nGeo Mean (95% CI)Detectable
Creatinine (mg/dL)51987.7 (83.3, 92.3)17364.7 (59.2, 70.7)17385.1 (78.7, 92.0)173122.7 (114.0, 132.0)<0.001
2,4-D (μg/L)3440.35 (0.33, 0.38)630.22 (0.21, 0.24)1270.27 (0.25, 0.29)1540.53 (0.46, 0.60)<0.001
 Multiple imputation5180.25 (0.24, 0.27)1730.19 (0.17, 0.21)1720.24 (0.22, 0.27)1730.36 (0.31, 0.42)<0.001
2,4-D (μg/g of creatinine)c5180.27 (0.25, 0.29)1730.21 (0.20, 0.23)1720.25 (0.22, 0.27)1730.36 (0.31, 0.42)<0.00166.2%
 Missing1010
 Above LOD3430.34 (0.31, 0.37)630.28 (0.24, 0.31)1270.29 (0.27, 0.33)1540.41 (0.35, 0.48)<0.001
 Below LOD1741104519
 Interfering substances0000
Glyphosate (μg/L)5100.87 (0.79, 0.95)1650.29 (0.26, 0.32)1730.91 (0.84, 0.98)1722.40 (2.15, 2.68)<0.001
Glyphosate (μg/g of creatinine)c5190.92 (0.83, 1.01)1730.38 (0.32, 0.45)1731.07 (0.96, 1.20)1731.90 (1.67, 2.17)<0.00198.3%
 Above LOD510165173172
 Below LOD (n)9801
 Interfering substances (n)0000
ECBA (μg/L)1761.54 (1.20, 1.99)520.24 (0.74, 1.72)601.80 (1.11, 2.92)1091.72 (1.13, 2.61)0.62
 Multiple imputation5180.17 (0.14, 0.20)1730.17 (0.13, 0.22)1720.19 (0.14, 0.26)1730.15 (0.31, 0.42)0.72
ECBA (μg/g of creatinine)c5180.36 (0.32, 0.41)1730.41 (0.34, 0.50)1720.39 (0.31, 0.51)1730.29 (0.23, 0.37)0.3333.4%
 Missing1010
 Above LOD1761.49 (1.15, 1.93)521.58 (1.02, 2.44)601.74 (1.07, 2.84)641.23 (0.81, 1.89)0.33
 Below LOD342121112109
 Interfering substances0000
DCBA (μg/L)3191.25 (1.04, 1.50)1040.98 (0.73, 1.32)1031.44 (1.02, 2.05)1121.37 (1.00, 1.88)0.64
 Multiple imputation5110.38 (0.31, 0.45)1720.40 (0.29, 0.53)1710.39 (0.29, 0.54)1680.34 (0.24, 0.47)0.73
DCBA (μg/g of creatinine)c5110.63 (0.55, 0.73)1720.71 (0.57, 0.89)1710.66 (0.51, 0.85)1680.53 (0.41, 0.68)0.2863.3%
 Missing1010
 Above LOD3191.22 (1.01, 1.47)1041.34 (0.98, 1.81)1031.40 (0.99, 1.99)1120.99 (0.72, 1.36)0.28
 Below LOD192686856
 Interfering substances7115
Note: Values presented are n (percent), mean±SD, median (25th percentile to 75th percentile), or geometric mean (95% CI). The herbicide summary score was calculated using the following steps. For each biomarker (2,4-D and glyphosate), observations below the LOD were imputed with a constant (LOD divided by the square root of two). One was added to the variable and was natural log-transformed. The log-transformed concentrations were divided by the group’s standard deviation and then averaged with biomarkers of the same classification. We calculated the p-value for trend (p-trend) for participant characteristics across tertiles of the herbicide summary score using unadjusted linear regression for continuous variables (continuous participant characteristic=herbicide summary score) and unadjusted logistic regression for categorical variables (categorical participant characteristic=herbicide summary score). —, no data; 2,4-D, 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid; BMI, body mass index; CI, confidence interval; DCBA, 3-(diethylcarbamoyl)benzoic acid; DEET, N,N-diethyl-meta-toluamide; dL, deciliter; ECBA, 3-(ethylcarbamoyl)benzoic acid; ESPINA, Secondary Exposures to Pesticides among Children and Adolescents; g, gram; Geo mean, geometric mean; L, liter; LOD, limit of detection; m, meter; mL, mililiter; pg, picogram; SD, standard deviation; U, units; μg, microgram; USD, U.S. dollars; y, years.
a
Household income was imputed for 11 adolescents. Five of these adolescents had household income imputed from what was reported from the 2008 examination. For the remaining 6 adolescents, income was imputed based on the average parental education (years). We calculated the cohort’s mean±SD income in 2016 for each year of average parental education. For those with missing income, we imputed the value based on a random normal distribution of the corresponding parental education.
b
Average parental education was imputed using the reported education from 2008 to impute father’s education for four participants and to impute mother’s education for five participants. For the remaining four participants with missing paternal education and three participants with missing maternal education, we used a random imputation based on a normal distribution of paternal education and maternal education, respectively. The average parental education was then calculated from the imputed values.
c
Creatinine adjusted. Values below the LOD were imputed using a constant (LOD divided by the square root of two). If a variable had missing observations, the sample size was provided. Missing was not used to calculate percentages.
Summary scores for herbicide and DEET were calculated using the following steps. For each biomarker, observations below the LOD were imputed with a constant (LOD divided by the square root of two81). One was added to the variable and was natural log-transformed. The log-transformed concentrations were divided by the group’s standard deviation and then averaged with biomarkers of the same classification. For the DEET summary score, ECBA and DCBA were combined while for the herbicide summary score, 2,4-D, and glyphosate were combined.
GEE models were used to analyze the relationship between each pesticide biomarker and summary score with the 5 NEPSY-II domains: attention & inhibitory control, language, memory & learning, visuospatial processing, and social perception. Confounders were identified a priori and were first assessed using a directed acyclic graph (DAG), and visualized using Daggity.82 Confounders that were considered included age (continuous in years), gender [male, female (reference)], race [Indigenous, White, or Mestizo (reference)], z-BMI-for-age (continuous), z-height-for-age (continuous), hemoglobin concentration (continuous), urinary creatinine concentration (continuous), tanner score (continuous), monthly salary (continuous in United States dollar), average parental education (continuous in years), participant education level (continuous in years), examination date, and living with an agricultural worker [yes, no (reference)]. In addition to using a DAG to identify potential confounders, and to limit parameters of our model, additional testing was conducted to determine if confounders were necessary such as using a generalized linear model or 10% change in estimate criteria. Further details are described below.
Using a directed acyclic graph (Figure S1), we achieved confounding control by adjusting for included age, gender, race, z-BMI-for-age, urinary creatinine concentration, monthly salary, and parental education. Age and gender helped to control for social determinants of pesticide exposure and neurobehavior, as pesticide exposure and performance on NEPSY-II may vary by age and gender. Differences in pesticide exposure and neurobehavior have been observed across different racial groups.83 Z-BMI-for-age is included as a marker of chronic and subacute nutritional status, and has been associated with neurobehavioral deficits.54 Z-BMI-for-age may also influence herbicide and DEET exposure, as adipose tissue is a known site of pesticide accumulation.84 Creatinine concentration helps to adjust for urine dilution when measuring pesticide metabolite concentrations.85 Monthly salary was adjusted in the model to control for confounding by economic factors.
Variables that were considered to be confounders but were identified to be unnecessary (Figure S1) include hemoglobin concentration, z-height-for-age, tanner staging, participant education level, living with an agricultural worker, examination date, and prenatal pesticide exposure. For hemoglobin concentration, iron deficiency has been inversely related to cognitive function and may have a negative impact on the developing brain86 but has not been known to influence pesticide exposure. Tanner staging was identified to be a mediator in our DAG, as pesticide exposure may influence puberty development87 and hormonal changes caused by puberty can affect neurobehavior.88 Participant education level was found to be highly correlated with age (r=0.81, p<0.0001). We only included age in our models to avoid collinearity with participant education; however, age in this study population can control for confounding by education, although with some amount of residual confounding. Living with an agricultural worker is a proxy pesticide exposure measure from take-home exposure. However, as this is an alternative pesticide exposure construct, it would introduce bias by partially adjusting for our exposure of interest. There was concern that participants would perform differently on neurobehavioral tests if they were evaluated during the summer vacation (July–August) than during the school year (October). We ran a general linear model (GLM) to determine whether there were performance differences between examination in October vs. July to August [1=October assessment, 0=JulyAugust examination (reference)] and each NEPSY-II domain score, adjusting for age, gender, race, and parental education. We did not find a statistically significant difference between the two time periods for attention & inhibitory control [0.70 (0.12, 1.53), p=0.09], language [0.22 (0.53, 0.98), p=0.56], memory & learning [0.69 (1.57, 0.20), p=0.13], visuospatial processing [0.72 (0.15, 1.61), p=0.11], or social perception [0.14 (1.05, 0.75), p=0.75]. Thus, the final model does not adjust for examination period (October vs. summer). There has been evidence that prenatal pesticide exposure may influence neurobehavioral performance in childhood.89 We used two constructs for prenatal pesticide exposure: a) mother worked in agriculture while pregnant with participant and b) mother cohabitated with an agricultural worker while pregnant with participant. After limiting the sample size to only include participants with the prenatal exposure constructs and adjusting our model for both prenatal pesticide exposure constructs, independently and together, we did not observe a 10% change in our beta estimates for the herbicide-neurobehavioral associations of interest for any of the domains (Table S1–S5). There were >10% changes for some of the associations between DEET metabolites and all domains (Table S1–S5). As the estimates for these associations were very small (close to null), and to maintain consistency across the models, we decided not to include it as a covariate.
Any models that had attention & inhibitory control as the outcome were also adjusted for a learning effect (test-retest),90 as participants were also examined for this domain in April 2016. For associations between biomarker and summary score concentrations with NEPSY-II scores, clustered the bootstrap method was used to obtain each p-value.91 This was done by running a GEE model for each ln-transformed biomarker or summary score concentration with each NEPSY-II outcome 1,000 times. We then obtained asymptotic estimates and p-values. Curvilinearity was assessed by testing squared (quadratic) terms of the pesticide biomarkers measured and summary scores in the adjusted linear models and were reported if they reached a threshold significance level of p<0.10.
To assess whether there were additive effects between both herbicides, interaction terms of continuous metabolite concentrations (i.e., 2,4-D×glyphosate) were introduced in the model and were considered if they had a p<0.10. If these effects were found to be statistically significant, effect modification on the multiplicative and additive scales were assessed. For the multiplicative scale, the association of one biomarker with the neurobehavioral domain was stratified across median splits of the second pesticide. To assess effect modification on the additive scale, we examined joint indicators using median splits of both biomarkers on the NEPSY-II domain [lower median of biomarker 1 and 2 (reference); upper/lower; lower/upper; upper/upper]. We then calculated the interaction contrast ratios (ICRs) and 95% CI to determine risk of having NEPSY-II scores below the expected level (score5).65 Effect modification by gender was assessed using a multiplicative interaction term for the herbicide or DEET metabolite concentrations associations with neurobehavioral domain scores. If the multiplicative term had a significance of p<0.10, the association was stratified by gender. Given that our exposure was log-transformed, we multiplied the β estimates by log(1.5) to determine the change in NEPSY-II score per 50% higher herbicide or DEET metabolite concentrations. Reported squared terms, however, were not back transformed.
Mediation analyses.
Using structural equation modeling (SEM), we assessed whether testosterone, 17β-estradiol, cortisol, or DHEA mediated the herbicides or DEET metabolites associated with neurobehavioral domain scores that were either statistically significant or borderline statistically significant (p<0.10) using the R lavaan package.9294
The SEM consisted of two regression modules, one relates the mediator (Z) to the predictor (X) and the other relates the outcome (Y) to the mediator (Z) and predictor (X). The modules are as follows:
Module 1:   Z=β0+β1X,
Module 2:  Y=r0+r1Z+r2X.
Module 1 regressed the herbicide or DEET metabolite concentration (predictor, X) on hormone concentration (mediator, Z) and Module 2 regressed the herbicide or DEET metabolite concentration (predictor, X) and hormone concentration (mediator, Z) with NEPSY outcomes (outcome, Y).95
The primary hypothesis concerns full mediation, r2=0, in which case Z fully mediates the effect of X on Y. Generally, Z mediates some (not all) of the effects of X on Y, in which case r2 is not zero. In such partial mediation cases, we compute the direct, indirect, and total effects, often expressed in percentages to indicate the degree to which Z mediates the effects of X on Y. Thus, in the case of full mediation, the direct effect is 0 and the indirect effect is 100%. The direct effect of pesticide exposure was estimated by the effect of the pesticide biomarkers on the outcome in Module 2. The indirect effect was estimated by cumulating the effect of the mediator on the outcome in the second module and the effect of pesticide biomarkers on mediator in the first module. Model fit was assessed by chi-square test, the comparative fit index, the index of Tucker and Lewis, and root mean square error of approximation.96,97 Mediation was determined to be present if the indirect effect was statistically significant. Mediation models were adjusted for retest learning effect, age, gender, race, creatinine, z-BMI-for-age, monthly salary, saliva collection time minus awakening time, and average parental education for the attention & inhibitory control outcome. All other mediation models were adjusted for the same covariates, minus retest learning effect. We found that our data met assumptions of SEM, which include normality of observations, no systematic missing data, no measurement or sampling and has a good model fit.98
Locally weighted polynomial regression (LOESS) curves graphs were created to visualize associations and to assess the presence of a threshold effect between 2,4-D and all 5 neurobehavioral domains and between glyphosate and social perception. The LOESS graphs plotted the associations and 95% CIs (smoothness factor: 0.9) using the fully adjusted least squares means of each domain score for 300 ranks of observed pesticide metabolite concentrations and ranks of imputed concentrations. All analyses were performed using R statistical programming software.

Results

Participant Characteristics

Participants had a balanced gender distribution (51.3% female) and 78.0% were Mestizo or White (Table 1). The average age of participants was 14.46 y old (SD=1.76), and the average tanner score was 2.89 (SD=0.95). In unadjusted analyses, participants with higher herbicide summary scores were more likely to be male, have higher urinary creatinine levels, higher education levels and have lower z-BMI-for-age (Table 1). The differences in gender [β=0.10 (0.04, 0.24), p=0.16] and z-BMI-for-age score [β=0.08 (0.17, 0.002), p=0.06] were no longer present after adjusting the GEE models for age, z-Height-for-age, and creatinine concentration. The differences in education level were not present when adjusted for age and gender [β=0.05 (0.13, 0.02), p=0.16]. The mean of each neurobehavioral domain score was between 7.0 and 8.7 (SD range: 2.0–2.3) (Table 2). Our study population is relatively representative of the population of Pedro Moncayo. Compared to the adolescents in the 2016 Community Health Survey of Pedro Moncayo, the ESPINA cohort had an equivalent gender distribution (ESPINA: 51.3% female vs. 2016 Survey: 50.8% female) and mean age [ESPINA: 14.47 (SD: 1.77) vs. 2016 Community Health Survey: 14.45 (SD: 2.27)]. However, there was a higher proportion of agricultural workers who were parents within ESPINA (67.5%) than adults between the ages of 26 and 74 years in the 2016 Community Health Survey (42.6%). There were low positive correlations (0<r0.32) with glyphosate, 2,4-D, DCBA, and ECBA with AND, PNP, TCPy, and 3-PBA (Table S6).
Table 2 Unadjusted mean neurobehavioral domain scores: overall and stratified by herbicide summary score medians of adolescent participants of the ESPINA study examination in the July to October 2016 examination (n=519).
 OverallHerbicide summary score
Lower median (0.17–0.99)Upper median (1.00–9.09)
Attention & inhibition8.38±2.158.42±2.088.31±2.23
Language7.04±2.047.16±1.956.91±2.13
Memory & learning8.30±2.288.43±2.308.19±2.27
Visuospatial processing8.69±2.288.66±2.228.71±2.35
Social perception8.23±2.348.44±2.308.02±2.36
Note: Estimates are mean±SD. The herbicide summary score was calculated using values for 2,4-D and glyphosate. Observations below the LOD were imputed, one was added, and the variable was natural log-transformed. The log-transformed concentrations were divided by the group’s standard deviation, and then the two biomarkers were averaged. 2,4-D, 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid; ESPINA, Secondary Exposures to Pesticides among Children and Adolescents; LOD, limit of detection; SD, standard deviation.
The urinary concentrations of herbicides and DEET metabolites, overall and across categories of the herbicide summary score, are listed in Table 1. The 2,4-D and DEET biomarkers were measured in the general population of adolescents in the United States in 2013–2014 or 2015–2016, respectively, as part of the NHANES. Compared to NHANES, our cohort’s geometric mean concentrations were higher for glyphosate [ESPINA: 0.85μg/g of creatinine (0.74, 0.97) vs. NHANES 2015–2016: 0.32μg/g (0.29, 0.36)], equivalent for 2,4-D [ESPINA: 0.27μg/g (95% CI: 0.25, 0.29) vs. NHANES 2013–2014: 0.29μg/g (0.26, 0.32)], but lower for DCBA [ESPINA: 0.63μg/g (0.55, 0.73) vs. NHANES 2015–2016: 4.42μg/g (3.48, 5.61)] and ECBA [ESPINA: 0.36μg/g (0.32, 0.41) vs. NHANES 2015–2016: 1.77μg/g (1.40, 2.23)] concentrations.79 Average NEPSY-II domain scores can be found in Table 2. Compared to the U.S. normative sample of the NEPSY-II, scaled scores among ESPINA participants were lower.

Urinary Pesticide Biomarker Concentrations and Neurobehavioral Performance

The DEET summary score and DCBA and ECBA concentrations were not found to be associated with any of the domains (Table 3). Herbicide metabolites, however, were found to be negatively associated with neurobehavior. Higher observed urinary concentrations of 2,4-D were associated with worse performance across the 5 neurobehavioral domains assessed. These associations were statistically significant for attention & inhibitory control [score difference per 50% higher metabolite concentration (βper50%)=0.19 (95% CI: 0.31, 0.07)] and language [βper50%=0.12 (0.23, 0.01)]. The associations were also negative, albeit borderline nonstatistically significant, for memory & learning [βper50%=0.11 (0.22, 0.01)] and nonstatistically significant for social perception [0.11 (0.27, 0.06)]; however, social perception had a statistically significant curvilinear association (pquadratic<0.01) (Table S7; Table 3, Figure 2). The numeric data for Figure 2 can be found in the supplemental excel file. Urinary glyphosate concentrations were negatively associated with social perception [βper50%=0.08 (0.14, 0.01)]. However, it was not associated with any other domain.
Table 3 Adjusted associations of pesticide metabolite concentrations or summary scores with neurobehavioral performance for participants of the July to October 2016 ESPINA study examination (n=519).
 nDifference in domain scores per 50% higher biomarker concentration, β (95% CI)
Attention & inhibitory controlaLanguagebMemory & learningbVisuospatial processingbSocial perceptionb
β (95% CI)p-Valueβ (95% CI)p-Valueβ (95% CI)p-Valueβ (95% CI)p-Valueβ (95% CI)p-Value
Herbicide summary score5190.09 (0.22, 0.05)c0.210.12 (0.25, 0.01)0.040.12 (0.25, 0.02)0.090.14 (0.29, 0.004)0.060.24 (0.39, 0.10)0.001
  2,4-D, observed5180.19 (0.31, 0.07)0.0020.12 (0.23, 0.01)0.030.11 (0.22, 0.01)0.080.05 (0.16, 0.06)0.400.11 (0.27, 0.06)d0.19
  2,4-D, imputed0.003 (0.16, 0.17)0.980.03 (0.20, 0.14)0.730.05 (0.14, 0.25)0.600.01 (0.18, 0.18)0.950.03 (0.17, 0.22)0.80
  Glyphosate5190.01 (0.05, 0.07)0.670.01 (0.07, 0.04)0.620.02 (0.09, 0.05)0.540.02 (0.09, 0.05)0.640.08 (0.14, 0.01)0.02
DEET summary score5180.01 (0.07, 0.08)0.860.01 (0.06, 0.09)0.720.01 (0.08, 0.10)0.780.06 (0.14, 0.02)0.130.01 (0.09, 0.11)0.86
  ECBA, observed5180.01 (0.06, 0.08)0.800.03 (0.11, 0.06)0.550.01 (0.10, 0.08)0.870.01 (0.08, 0.07)0.870.04 (0.13, 0.05)0.38
  ECBA, imputed0.01 (0.05, 0.06)0.840.003 (0.05, 0.05)0.900.01 (0.07, 0.05)0.680.001 (0.06, 0.06)0.980.001 (0.06, 0.06)0.98
  DCBA, observed5180.01 (0.05, 0.06)0.720.01 (0.07, 0.05)0.750.01 (0.06, 0.07)0.860.04 (0.09, 0.02)0.230.02 (0.09, 0.05)0.61
  DCBA, imputed0.01 (0.06, 0.08)0.830.01 (0.06, 0.08)0.790.01 (0.07, 0.09)0.770.02 (0.06, 0.09)0.700.02 (0.06, 0.10)0.68
Note: Results in the table are from linear exposure models except for associations from models with both linear and quadratic terms for exposure, such as those seen in footnote c and d. The p-values are from general estimating equations that were used to analyze the relationship between each pesticide biomarker or summary score with each NEPSY-II domain, adjusting for confounders. A p-value<0.05 indicates a statistically significant association between the tested biomarker and NEPSY-II domain. Summary scores for herbicide and DEET were calculated using the following steps: For each biomarker, observations below the LOD were imputed with a constant (LOD/2). One was added to the variable and was natural log-transformed. The log-transformed concentrations were divided by the group’s standard deviation and then averaged with biomarkers of the same classification. For the DEET summary score, ECBA and DCBA were combined. For the herbicide summary score, 2,4-D and glyphosate were combined. All curvilinear terms can be found in Table S7. 2,4-D, DCBA, and ECBA concentrations below the LOD were imputed using multiple imputation. Imputation using a constant (LOD/2) was done for glyphosate. 2,4-D, 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid; BMI, body mass index; CI, confidence interval; DCBA, 3-(diethylcarbamoyl)benzoic acid; DEET, N,N-diethyl-meta-toluamide; ECBA, 3-(ethylcarbamoyl)benzoic acid; ESPINA, Secondary Exposures to Pesticides among Children and Adolescents; LOD, limit of detection; SD, standard deviation.
a
Model adjusts for retest learning effect, age, gender, race, creatinine, z-BMI-for-age, monthly salary, and average parental education.
b
Model adjusts for covariates of model A except for retest learning effect.
c
Curvilinear associations: β2=0.12 [(0.25, 0.01), p=0.07], β=0.36 (p=0.03). β and β2 listed are part of the same model.
d
Curvilinear associations: β2=0.21 [(0.09, 0.34), p<0.01], β=0.21 [(0.47, 0.05), p=0.11]. β and β2 listed are part of the same model.
Figure 2. Adjusted associations of 2,4-D concentrations with neurobehavioral domains, and glyphosate concentration with social perception of participants of the July to October 2016 ESPINA study examination (n=519). Each data point represents the adjusted least squares of NEPSY-II domain for 300 ranks of ln(metabolite). Blue triangles represent imputed values, while red dots represent observed values. Attention & inhibitory control adjusts for retest learning effect, age, gender, race, creatinine, z-BMI-for-age, monthly salary, and average parental education. All additional models adjust for the aforementioned model covariates except for retest learning effect. The gray line represents the LOESS line, while the outer bands are the 95% confidence intervals across each value of the x-axis. Numeric data for this figure can be found in the Supplemental Excel File. Note: 2,4-D, 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid; BMI, body mass index; ESPINA, Secondary Exposures to Pesticides among Children and Adolescents; L, liter; LOESS, locally weighted polynomial regression; μg, microgram.
The herbicide summary score had negative associations with language [βper50%=0.12 (0.25, 0.01)] and social perception [βper50%=0.24 (0.39, 0.10)], and borderline statistically significant negative associations with memory & learning [βper50%=0.12 (0.25, 0.02)] and visuospatial processing [βper50%=0.14 (0.29, 0.004)] (Table 3). The interaction terms between glyphosate and 2,4-D were statistically significant for memory & learning [β=0.14 (0.01, 0.27), p=0.04], but not for attention & inhibitory control [0.01 (0.14, 0.16), p=0.89], language [0.04 (0.05, 0.14), p=0.37], visuospatial processing [0.07 (0.23, 0.09), p=0.39], or social perception [0.10 (0.04, 0.24), p=0.15]. For the median splits, the range for creatinine adjusted 2,4-D with imputation using a constant was 0.04μg/L to 0.253μg/L in the lower median, and 0.254μg/L to 57.60μg/L for the upper median. While for creatinine adjusted glyphosate with imputation using a constant, the range was 0.001μg/L to 1.001μg/L in the lower median, and 1.002μg/L to 21.94μg/L in the upper median (Table 4). The association between glyphosate and memory & learning was stronger in the upper median of 2,4-D [βper50%=0.06 (0.19, 0.07), p=0.38] compared to the lower median [βper50%=0.001 (0.07, 0.07), p=0.99]. The association between 2,4-D and memory & learning was stronger and more negative in the upper median of glyphosate concentration [βper50%=0.10 (0.21, 0.02), p=0.10] compared to concentrations in the lower median [βper50%=0.06 (0.23, 0.10), p=0.45]. Participants who had concentrations in the upper median of both 2,4-D and glyphosate had lower scores in memory & learning by 0.55 (95% CI: 1.11, 0.01; p=0.05) compared to those in the lower median of both 2,4-D and glyphosate concentration (Table 5); the ICR for a below-expected memory & learning score was not statistically significant (1.52; 95% CI: 1.94, 4.98). No other interactions between metabolites were observed.
Table 4 Adjusted associations of pesticide metabolite concentrations with memory & learning performance across strata of a secondary pesticide metabolite median split for participants of the July to October 2016 ESPINA study examination (n=518).
 Difference in domain scores per 50% higher biomarker concentration, β (95% CI)
2,4-DGlyphosate
Lower median (0.040.253μg/L)Upper median (0.25457.60μg/L)Lower median (0.0011.001μg/L)Upper median (1.00221.94μg/L)
β (95% CI)p-Valueβ (95% CI)p-Valueβ (95% CI)p-Valueβ (95% CI)p-Value
2,4-D0.06 (0.23, 0.10)0.450.10 (0.21, 0.02)0.10
Glyphosate0.001 (0.07, 0.07)0.990.06 (0.19, 0.07)0.38
Note: β estimates were obtained by running generalized estimating equations to test the association of one biomarker with the neurobehavioral domain stratified across median splits of the second pesticide. A p<0.05 indicates a statistically significant association. Models adjusts for age, gender, race, creatinine, z-BMI-for-age, monthly salary, and average parental education. 2,4-D and glyphosate concentrations below the LOD were imputed using a constant (LOD/2). 2,4-D, 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid; BMI, body mass index; CI, confidence interval; ESPINA, Secondary Exposures to Pesticides among Children and Adolescents; L, liter; LOD, limit of detection; μg, microgram.
Table 5 The memory & learning score differences for the joint effects of 2,4-D and glyphosate biomarker concentrations in adolescent participants of the ESPINA study, July to October 2016 examination (n=518).
2,4-D, μg/LGlyphosate, μg/LScore difference (95% CI), p-Value
median: 0.04–0.253median: 0.001–1.001Reference
>median: 0.254–57.60median: 0.001–1.0010.15 (0.73, 0.43), 0.61
median: 0.04–0.253>median: 1.002–21.940.16 (0.73, 0.41). 0.58
>median: 0.254–57.60>median: 1.002–21.940.55 (1.11, 0.01), 0.05
Note: The model adjusted for retest learning effect, age, gender, race, creatinine, z-BMI-for-age, monthly salary, and average parental education. The score difference estimates were obtained by conducting generalized estimating equations to assess the association between an indicator variable based on dichotomous variables for 2,4-D and glyphosate concentrations, and memory & learning scores. A p-value<0.05 indicates that the respective category (e.g., upper median of both 2,4-D and glyphosate) is statistically significantly different than the reference group (lower median of 2,4-D and glyphosate). 2,4-D and glyphosate concentrations below the LOD were imputed using a constant (LOD/2). 2,4-D, 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid; BMI, body mass index; CI, confidence interval; ESPINA, Secondary Exposures to Pesticides among Children and Adolescents; LOD, limit of detection.
Additionally, no effect modification by gender was observed on any of the associations between any of the metabolites and neurobehavioral performance (Table S8).

Mediation by Gender and Adrenal Hormones

We investigated mediation by DHEA, testosterone, estradiol (measured in boys only) and cortisol on the observed statistically significant associations between pesticide biomarker concentrations with neurobehavioral outcomes, namely, glyphosate with social perception and 2,4-D with attention & inhibitory control, language, memory & learning, and social perception. There was no evidence of mediation in any of the models (Table S9).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the association between urinary glyphosate concentration and neurobehavioral performance in humans. In our study of adolescents growing up in agricultural settings, we observed that greater concentrations of the herbicide 2,4-D were associated with lower performance on four of the five neurobehavioral domains assessed: attention & inhibitory control, language, memory & learning, and social perception. Additionally, urinary glyphosate concentration was only inversely associated with social perception, whereas the two DEET metabolites were not associated with neurobehavioral outcomes. Combined concentrations of 2,4-D and glyphosate, as assessed by an herbicide summary score, was associated with worse performance on all five neurobehavioral domains assessed. These findings suggest that exposure to these two herbicides may negatively impact neurobehavior in adolescents. Within our cohort, concentrations of 2,4-D are equivalent to those of a general population of adolescents in the United States,79 and concentrations of glyphosate were higher than those reported in NHANES.79 Although we identified differences in herbicide exposure by gender in crude analysis (Table 1), this difference disappeared after further adjustment was made.
Few epidemiological studies have investigated the association between 2,4-D exposure and cognitive development; none of the studies were conducted in adolescents. Two studies reported negative effects of 2,4-D on neurobehavior. In a study of 232 infants in Southeast China, prenatal exposure to 2,4-D was assessed by measuring its concentration in umbilical cord blood plasma, and they reported deficits in auditory processing among infants with prenatal exposure [0.103 (95% BCI: 0.24, 0)].27 A study of workers in Jacksonville, Florida found that active, retired and former workers of a plant that manufactured 2,4-D had a statistically significant decrease in nerve conduction velocity compared to nonherbicide factory workers (mean=34.0 vs. 40.1m/s, p<0.02).28 However, among 1,338 adult participants of the NHANES III conducted in the United States, there were no associations from adjusted linear regression models between 2,4-D urinary concentrations and concurrent performance on three neurobehavioral tests: simple reaction time (β=3.86, p=0.62), symbol digit substitution [mean total latency (β=0.93, p=0.44) and number of errors (β=0.0007, p=0.99)], and serial digit learning [trials to criterion (β=0.39, p=0.19) and total score (β=1.03, p=0.10)].26 The differing findings between ESPINA and NHANES may be due to multiple factors. Compared to adults, adolescents have increased susceptibility to insecticide neurotoxicity,99 which may also be true for 2,4-D. Brain development and performance on neurobehavioral tests can vary across different stages of life. For these two reasons, adults and adolescents may not be the most comparable groups. Furthermore, the neurobehavioral domains evaluated in NHANES III26 (simple reaction time, symbol digit substitution, and serial digit learning) are not comparable to those measured in this study (attention & inhibitory control, language, memory & learning, visuospatial processing, and social perception). It is plausible that 2,4-D may affect certain neurodevelopmental domains more than others.
To our knowledge, there are no studies which focus on the effects of 2,4-D and its effects on the nervous system in adolescents. However, it is possible that the associations that we observed between 2,4-D exposure and cognition may be due to its neurotoxic effects. 2,4-D has been found to induce neuronal and glial cell death and neuronal necrosis in rodents100,101 due to an increase in reactive oxygen species and free radicals.102 Additionally, rats that were exposed orally and via inhalation to 2,4-D for 6 months had increased expression of BAX, a B-2 cell lymphoma pro-apoptotic molecule, and had a greater incidence of individual neuron necrosis and lower cerebral cortex thickness compared to the rat control group.103 The rats exposed to 2,4-D had reduced cognitive ability, measured using object recognition and impaired exploration behaviors.104 Each of these neurotoxic events have been shown to negatively affect behaviors, such as locomotion among animal subjects.105,106
We observed a statistically significant inverse association between glyphosate and facial affect recognition (social perception). This is the first time this association has been characterized in an epidemiologic study, to our knowledge. Recent evidence from epidemiological and animal studies suggests potential links between glyphosate exposure and ASD.24,25 ASD is a condition that is associated with reduced emotion recognition and is evidenced by lower scores on the Affect recognition subtest (social perception domain, NEPSY-2).107111 In a systematic review based on two epidemiological and 15 in vivo studies, glyphosate showed a moderate level of evidence with an increased risk of ASD in children.25 Prenatal exposures to pesticides (measured as pounds of pesticides applied per acre/month within 2,000m from the maternal residence) was associated with increased risk of ASD when compared to the offspring of women from the same agricultural region in California without exposure [OR=1.16 (95% CI: 1.06, 1.27) for glyphosate].24 Elevated intracellular chloride levels of neurons and neocortical tissue have been observed in autism.112,113 Glycine binding of the glycine receptor of neurons can influence the rate of calcium influx into neurons during neurodevelopment.110,114 Given glyphosate’s potential to be a glycine mimetic, it could cause similar patterns of overconcentrated chloride within immature neurons.110 We could not identify any other published epidemiologic studies that evaluated the associations between glyphosate urinary concentration and neurobehavioral outcomes in a human population.

Mediation by Gender and Adrenal Hormones

Although there is evidence of the endocrine disrupting potential of 2,4-D and glyphosate,44,45,115 we did not observe evidence of mediation by the gonadal or adrenal hormones assessed on the herbicide-neurobehavior associations. There is biological evidence in animal and in vivo studies suggesting that pesticide exposure may influence endocrine alterations. Glyphosate has been found to have 8 of 10 key characteristics of being an endocrine disrupting chemical, including, but not limited to, interacting with hormone receptors and altering hormone receptor expression.46 2,4-D has been considered a potential endocrine disrupting chemical through its effects on gonadal morphology and testosterone production.116 Factors that may have influenced our inability to detect mediating effects of hormones are the notable variability of sex hormones across puberty stages and our limited ability to evaluate temporality (the pesticide exposure leading to an endocrine change and then to a neurobehavioral alteration). It is plausible that mediation effects may be most detectable and valid if they are assessed longitudinally. Studies conducting longitudinal measures of pesticides, neurobehavior, and hormone concentrations during adolescence are warranted. Likewise, incorporating biological markers of puberty stage, like luteinizing or follicle stimulating hormones could have improved our models. Additionally, we did not observe any effect modification by gender among these adolescent participants, unlike previous findings in this cohort, when participants were children.67

Multiple Exposure

We saw evidence of effect modification on both the additive and multiplicative scales between both 2,4-D and glyphosate herbicides with memory & learning. In the lower median of 2,4-D concentrations, higher glyphosate concentration had a stronger and more negative association with memory & learning than in the upper median, while in the lower median of glyphosate concentration, 2,4-D had a more negative association with memory & learning. No studies currently exist that have examined the effect measure modification of these herbicides on neurobehavioral outcomes; however, some studies have looked into the joint effects of glyphosate and 2,4-D in phytoplankton and periphyton and genotoxicity in fish. Lozano et al. found that there were additive effects of the combined herbicides, as the effects of the mixture did not exceed the estimated combined effects from the individual chemicals in terms of toxicity.117 while a synergistic effect was observed among a combination of glyphosate and 2,4-D in terms of genotoxicity in a certain species of fish (Cnesterodon decemmaculatus).118

Strengths and Limitations

This is among the largest studies of adolescents to have assessed the effects of pesticide exposures and neurobehavior, using both urinary biomarker concentrations of pesticide exposures and neurobehavioral performance testing. A limitation is that the quantification of exposure biomarkers occurred at a single point in time. Urinary biomarker concentrations reflect recent exposure to the herbicides and insect repellants measured, as glyphosate has a half-life of 3.5–14.5 h,119 2,4-D has a half-life of 11.6 h,120 and DEET has a half-life of 4 h.121 However, it is plausible that these spot concentrations may be correlated with the yearly pesticide exposures given that agricultural production in this equatorial location occurs year-round. A prior analysis of this cohort assessed the relationship between home proximity to the nearest agricultural greenhouse and AChE exposure longitudinally (2008, spring 2016, and summer 2016 assessments).122 Using repeated measure regression, home proximity was negatively associated with AChE concentration, suggesting that there was potential pesticide exposure across all three time points, which indicates that there is year-round exposure to pesticides.122 This exposure may vary, as floricultural production does fluctuate seasonally depending on the demand for flowers for holidays like Thanksgiving, Christmas, Valentine’s day, and Mother’s day. Thus, periods of high pesticide use generally ranges from October to May, while June to September has lower pesticide use. Although the summer assessment occurred in a period of lower pesticide applications in floriculture, we still saw associates between 2,4-D and glyphosate with neurobehavioral outcomes. Another consideration is that herbicides are used sparingly in floriculture, as they can also lead to the destruction of rose crops, but are more commonly used in agriculture for crops like corn, wheat, and beans.55,123 Pedro Moncayo has year-round agriculture and subsequent pesticide application due to its tempered climate, equatorial location, and adequate irrigation water supply. This association needs to be further evaluated in this population using multiple measures of exposure. An additional limitation is our inability to distinguish whether the effects that we observed are due to short-term or long-term exposure to pesticides due to the cross-sectional analyses of this study. Lastly, it is possible that some of the results are influenced by co-exposures to other pesticides. However, we found low positive correlations (0<r0.32) across all 4 metabolites presented in this study with urinary metabolite concentrations of AND (neonicotinoid), PNP (organophosphate), TCPy (organophosphate), and 3-PBA (pyrethroid).
To improve causal inference, future studies should analyze changes in neurobehavioral development over time based on initial or time-varying exposures. Finally, we believe that our results for 2,4-D may be generalizable to other adolescent, nonoccupational populations given that we observed similar 2,4-D urinary concentrations in ESPINA as in the U.S. general population (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2022). Glyphosate concentrations were higher in our cohort compared to reported concentrations of adolescents in the United States.79 Therefore, additional research is needed to assess whether a similar association with social perception would still be detectable in adolescent populations with lower glyphosate exposure. Differences between glyphosate concentrations observed in ESPINA vs. NHANES may be due to differential patterns of herbicide use between the two regions that may be specific to the types of crops grown. DEET concentrations were lower in our cohort compared to NHANES, which may be why we did not detect any associations with the NEPSY-II domains. The lower DEET concentrations may be due to reduced use of insect repellents in the Ecuadorian highlands, where ESPINA participants live (2,800m above sea level), since there is a lower prevalence of mosquitoes and lower concern of insect-borne diseases compared to coastal regions.124 Individuals living in the highlands, such as Pedro Moncayo, may use less insect repellents compared to the U.S. NHANES population, which contains regions of lower altitude and higher risk of insect-borne diseases.

Conclusion

Urinary concentrations of 2,4-D and glyphosate were associated with lower scores in attention & inhibitory control, language, visuospatial processing, memory & learning, and social perception among adolescents in rural Ecuador. Independently, urinary 2,4-D concentration was negatively (statistically significantly or borderline statistically significantly) associated with attention & inhibitory control, language, visuospatial processing, and memory & learning, whereas urinary glyphosate concentration was inversely associated only with social perception. We did not identify any mediating effects by gonadal and adrenal hormones on the associations between herbicide or DEET exposure on neurobehavioral outcomes. These are among the first population-based findings to describe lower neurobehavioral performance associated with urinary concentrations of 2,4-D and glyphosate. Replication of these findings is needed, including longitudinal studies and assessments of joint effects of herbicide mixtures among various pediatric and adult populations.

Acknowledgments

We thank ESPINA study staff, Fundación Cimas del Ecuador, the Parish Governments of Pedro Moncayo County, community members of Pedro Moncayo and the Education District of Pichincha-Cayambe-Pedro Moncayo counties for their contributions and support on this project. We thank M. Ospina and A.M. Calafat for their contributions in quantifying most urinary pesticide metabolites and for assisting in preparation of this manuscript. We also thank A. Contreras and E. Gonzalez for their contributions on manuscript preparation and submission.
Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under award numbers R01ES025792, R01ES030378, R21ES026084, U2CES026560, and P30ES019776. B.N.C. Chronister was funded by the Institute of Mental Health (5T32MH122376).

Article Notes

R.C.M. is a co-founder of KeyWise, Inc. and a Consultant for NeuroUX. All other authors declare they have nothing to disclose.

Supplementary Material

File (ehp11383.s001.original.acco.pdf)
File (ehp11383.smcontents.508.pdf)
File (ehp11383.s001.acco.pdf)
File (ehp11383.s002.codeanddata.acco.zip)

References

1.
Benbrook CM. 2016. Trends in glyphosate herbicide use in the United States and globally. Environ Sci Eur 28(1):3, https://doi.org/10.1186/s12302-016-0070-0.
2.
Zuanazzi NR, Ghisi NDC, Oliveira EC. 2020. Analysis of global trends and gaps for studies about 2,4-D herbicide toxicity: a scientometric review. Chemosphere 241:125016. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31683446/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chemosphere.2019.125016.
3.
Song Y. 2014. Insight into the mode of action of 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid (2,4-D) as an herbicide. J Integr Plant Biol 56(2):106–113. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24237670/, https://doi.org/10.1111/jipb.12131.
4.
Duke SO. 2018. The history and current status of glyphosate. Pest Manag Sci 74(5):1027–1034. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28643882/, https://doi.org/10.1002/ps.4652.
5.
Powles SB. 2008. Evolved glyphosate-resistant weeds around the world: lessons to be learnt. Pest Manag Sci 64(4):360–365. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18273881/, https://doi.org/10.1002/ps.1525.
6.
Myers JP, Antoniou MN, Blumberg B, Carroll L, Colborn T, Everett LG, et al. 2016. Concerns over use of glyphosate-based herbicides and risks associated with exposures: a consensus statement. Environ Health 15(1):19. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26883814/, https://doi.org/10.1186/s12940-016-0117-0.
7.
NTP (National Toxicology Program). 1999. N,N-Diethyl-m-toluamide (DEET) [134-62-3]. Review of Toxicological Literature. https://ntp.niehs.nih.gov/sites/default/files/ntp/htdocs/chem_background/exsumpdf/deet_508.pdf [accessed 25 September 2023].
8.
Heydari N, Larsen D, Neira M, Beltrán Ayala E, Fernandez P, Adrian J, et al. 2017. Household dengue prevention interventions, expenditures, and barriers to Aedes aegypti control in Machala, Ecuador. Int J Environ Res Public Health 14(2):196, https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14020196.
9.
Bàrcena A, Katz J, Morales C, Shacper M. 2004. Los Transgenicos En America Latina y El Caribe: Un Debate Abierto. Santiago, Chile: Comisión Económica para América Latina y el Caribe (CEPAL).
10.
Ait-Bali Y, Ba-M’hamed S, Gambarotta G, Sassoè-Pognetto M, Giustetto M, Bennis M. 2020. Pre- and postnatal exposure to glyphosate-based herbicide causes behavioral and cognitive impairments in adult mice: evidence of cortical ad hippocampal dysfunction. Arch Toxicol 94(5):1703–1723. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32067069/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00204-020-02677-7.
11.
Gallegos CE, Bartos M, Bras C, Gumilar F, Antonelli MC, Minetti A. 2016. Exposure to a glyphosate-based herbicide during pregnancy and lactation induces neurobehavioral alterations in rat offspring. Neurotoxicology 53:20–28. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26632987/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2015.11.015.
12.
Coullery R, Pacchioni AM, Rosso SB. 2020. Exposure to glyphosate during pregnancy induces neurobehavioral alterations and downregulation of Wnt5a-CaMKII pathway. Reprod Toxicol 96:390–398. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32805371/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.reprotox.2020.08.006.
13.
Baier CJ, Gallegos CE, Raisman-Vozari R, Minetti A. 2017. Behavioral impairments following repeated intranasal glyphosate-based herbicide administration in mice. Neurotoxicol Teratol 64:63–72. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29061523/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2017.10.004.
14.
Faria M, Bedrossiantz J, Ramírez JRR, Mayol M, García GH, Bellot M, et al. 2021. Glyphosate targets fish monoaminergic systems leading to oxidative stress and anxiety. Environ Int 146:106253. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33220538/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2020.106253.
15.
Stürtz N, Deis RP, Jahn GA, Duffard R, Evangelista de Duffard AM. 2008. Effect of 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid on rat maternal behavior. Toxicology 247(2–3):73–79. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18420331/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tox.2008.02.001.
16.
Schulze GE, Dougherty JA. 1988. Neurobehavioral toxicity and tolerance to the herbicide 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid-n-butyl ester (2,4-D ester). Fundam Appl Toxicol 10(3):413–424. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/3371581/, https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-0590(88)90287-4.
17.
Squibb RE, Tilson HA, Mitchell CL. 1983. Neurobehavioral assessment of 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid (2,4-D) in rats. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 5(3):331–335. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/6877474/.
18.
Bortolozzi A, Duffard RO, Rubio M, Sturtz N, Evangelista De Duffard AM. 1998. Regionally specific changes in Central brain monoamine levels by 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid in acute treated rats. Neurotoxicology 19(6):839–852.
19.
Gaaied S, Oliveira M, Domingues I, Banni M. 2020. 2,4-Dichlorophenoxyacetic acid herbicide effects on zebrafish larvae: development, neurotransmission and behavior as sensitive endpoints. Environ Sci Pollut Res Int 27(4):3686–3696. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30778938/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11356-019-04488-5.
20.
Schoenig GP, Hartnagel RE, Schardein JL, Vorhees CV. 1993. Neurotoxicity evaluation of N,N-diethyl-m-toluamide (DEET) in rats. Toxicol Sci 21(3):355–365, https://doi.org/10.1093/toxsci/21.3.355.
21.
Abdel-Rahman A, Dechkovskaia AM, Goldstein LB, Bullman SH, Khan W, El-Masry EM, et al. 2004. Neurological deficits induced by malathion, DEET, and permethrin, alone or in combination in adult rats. J Toxicol Environ Health A 67(4):331–356. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14713564/, https://doi.org/10.1080/15287390490273569.
22.
Abou-Donia MB, Dechkovskaia AM, Goldstein LB, Abdel-Rahman A, Bullman SL, Khan WA. 2004. Co-exposure to pyridostigmine bromide, DEET, and/or permethrin causes sensorimotor deficit and alterations in brain acetylcholinesterase activity. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 77(2):253–262. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14751452/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2003.10.018.
23.
Fuhrimann S, Farnham A, Staudacher P, Atuhaire A, Manfioletti T, Niwagaba CB, et al. 2021. Exposure to multiple pesticides and neurobehavioral outcomes among smallholder farmers in Uganda. Environ Int 152:106477. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33756429/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2021.106477.
24.
von Ehrenstein OS, Ling C, Cui X, Cockburn M, Park AS, Yu F, et al. 2019. Prenatal and infant exposure to ambient pesticides and autism spectrum disorder in children: population based case-control study. BMJ 364:l962–10, https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l962.
25.
Ongono JS, Béranger R, Baghdadli A, Mortamais M. 2020. Pesticides used in Europe and autism spectrum disorder risk: can novel exposure hypotheses be formulated beyond organophosphates, organochlorines, pyrethroids and carbamates? A systematic review. Environ Res 187:109646. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32460093/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2020.109646.
26.
Krieg EF. 2013. The relationships between pesticide metabolites and neurobehavioral test performance in the third national health and nutrition examination survey. Arch Environ Occup Health 68(1):39–46. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23298423/, https://doi.org/10.1080/19338244.2011.633125.
27.
Silver MK, Shao J, Li M, Ji C, Chen M, Xia Y, et al. 2019. Prenatal exposure to the herbicide 2,4-D is associated with deficits in auditory processing during infancy. Environ Res 172:486–494. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30851698/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2019.02.046.
28.
Singer R, Moses M, Valciukas J, Lilis R, Selikoff IJ. 1982. Nerve conduction velocity studies of workers employed in the manufacture of phenoxy herbicides. Environ Res 29(2):297–311. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/7160349/, https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-9351(82)90032-9.
29.
Keith S, Harper C, Ashizawa AAA. 2017. Toxicology profile for DEET (N,N-Diethyl-Meta-Toluamide). 18. https://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/toxprofiles/tp185.pdf [accessed 16 February 2022].
30.
Osimitz TG, Murphy JV. 1997. Neurological effects associated with use of the insect repellent N,N-diethyl-m-toluamide (DEET). J Toxicol Clin Toxicol 35(5):435–441. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9279298/, https://doi.org/10.3109/15563659709001224.
31.
Legeay S, Clere N, Apaire-Marchais V, Faure S, Lapied B. 2018. Unusual modes of action of the repellent DEET in insects highlight some human side effects. Eur J Pharmacol 825:92–98. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29477656/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2018.02.033.
32.
Costas-Ferreira C, Dur R, Faro LRF. 2022. Toxic effects of glyphosate on the nervous system: a systematic review. Int J Mol Sci 23(9):4605, https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms23094605.
33.
Fornari E, Knyazeva MG, Meuli R, Maeder P. 2007. Myelination shapes functional activity in the developing brain. Neuroimage 38(3):511–518. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17889561/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.010.
34.
Pujol J, Soriano-Mas C, Ortiz H, Sebastián-Gallés N, Losilla JM, Deus J. 2006. Myelination of language-related areas in the developing brain. Neurology 66(3):339–343. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16476931/, https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000201049.66073.8d.
35.
Abd-Ella A, Stankiewicz M, Mikulska K, Nowak W, Pennetier C, Goulu M, et al. 2015. The repellent DEET potentiates carbamate effects via insect muscarinic receptor interactions: an alternative strategy to control insect vector-borne diseases. PLoS One 10(5):e0126406, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126406.
36.
Corbel V, Stankiewicz M, Pennetier C, Fournier D, Stojan J, Girard E, et al. 2009. Evidence for inhibition of cholinesterases in insect and mammalian nervous systems by the insect repellent deet. BMC Biol 7:47. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19656357/, https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7007-7-47.
37.
Herrington JE. 2004. Risk perceptions regarding ticks and Lyme disease: a national survey. Am J Prev Med 26(2):135–140. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14751325/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2003.10.010.
38.
Swale DR, Sun B, Tong F, Bloomquist JR. 2014. Neurotoxicity and mode of action of N, N-diethyl-Meta-toluamide (DEET). PLoS One 9(8):e103713. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25101788/, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103713.
39.
Weiss B. 2011. Endocrine disruptors as a threat to neurological function. J Neurol Sci 305(1–2):11–21. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/21474148/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2011.03.014.
40.
Yu J. 2014. Endocrine disorders and the neurologic manifestations. Ann Pediatr Endocrinol Metab 19(4):184–190. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25654063/, https://doi.org/10.6065/apem.2014.19.4.184.
41.
McEwen BS. 2012. Chapter 55 - Endocrine effects on the brain and their relationship to behavior. In: Basic Neurochemistry: Principles of Molecular, Cellular, and Medical Neurobiology. Brady ST, Siegel GJ, Albers RW, eds. 8th ed. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, 945–962.
42.
Schantz SL, Widholm JJ. 2001. Cognitive effects of endocrine-disrupting chemicals in animals. Environ Health Perspect 109(12):1197–1206. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11748026/, https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.011091197.
43.
Thongprakaisang S, Thiantanawat A, Rangkadilok N, Suriyo T, Satayavivad J. 2013. Glyphosate induces human breast cancer cells growth via estrogen receptors. Food Chem Toxicol 59(6):129–136. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23756170/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fct.2013.05.057.
44.
Mnif W, Hassine AIH, Bouaziz A, Bartegi A, Thomas O, Roig B. 2011. Effect of endocrine disruptor pesticides: a review. Int J Environ Res Public Health 8(6):2265–2303. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/21776230/, https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph8062265.
45.
Agostini LP, Dettogni RS, Dos Reis RS, Stur E, Dos Santos EVW, Ventorim DP, et al. 2020. Effects of glyphosate exposure on human health: insights from epidemiological and in vitro studies. Sci Total Environ 705:135808. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31972943/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2019.135808.
46.
Muñoz JP, Bleak TC, Calaf GM. 2021. Glyphosate and the key characteristics of an endocrine disruptor: a review. Chemosphere 270:128619. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33131751/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chemosphere.2020.128619.
47.
Ma Y, Han J, Guo Y, Lam PKS, Wu RSS, Giesy JP, et al. 2012. Disruption of endocrine function in in vitro H295R cell-based and in in vivo assay in zebrafish by 2,4-dichlorophenol. Aquat Toxicol 106–107:173–181. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22155427/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquatox.2011.11.006.
48.
Sundukov YN. 2006. First record of the ground beetle Trechoblemus postilenatus (Coleoptera, Carabidae) in Primorskii krai. Far East Entomol 165:16.
49.
Recio R, Ocampo-Gómez G, Morán-Martínez J, Borja-Aburto V, López-Cervante M, Uribe M, et al. 2005. Pesticide exposure alters follicle-stimulating hormone levels in Mexican agricultural workers. Environ Health Perspect 113(9):1160–1163. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16140621/, https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.7374.
50.
Dehnert GK, Karasov WH, Wolman MA. 2019. 2,4-Dichlorophenoxyacetic acid containing herbicide impairs essential visually guided behaviors of larval fish. Aquat Toxicol 209:1–12. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30684730/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquatox.2019.01.015.
51.
Suarez-Lopez JR, Nguyen A, Klas J, Gahagan S, Checkoway H, Lopez-Paredes D, et al. 2021. Associations of acetylcholinesterase inhibition between pesticide spray seasons with depression and anxiety symptoms in adolescents, and the role of sex and adrenal hormones on gender moderation. Expo Health 13(1):51–64. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33748533/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s12403-020-00361-w.
52.
Chronister BN, Gonzalez E, Lopez-Paredes D, Suarez-Torres J, Gahagan S, Martinez D, et al. 2021. Testosterone, estradiol, DHEA and cortisol in relation to anxiety and depression scores in adolescents. J Affect Disord 294:838–846. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/34375211/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.07.026.
53.
Suarez-Lopez JR, Jacobs DR, Himes JH, Alexander BH, Lazovich D, Gunnar M. 2012. Lower acetylcholinesterase activity among children living with flower plantation workers. Environ Res 114(1):53–59. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22405996/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2012.01.007.
54.
Grandjean P, Harari R, Barr DB, Debes F. 2006. Pesticide exposure and stunting as independent predictors of neurobehavioral deficits in Ecuadorian school children. Pediatrics 117(3):e546–e556. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16510633/, https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-1781.
55.
Suarez-Lopez JR, Butcher CR, Gahagan S, Checkoway H, Alexander BH, Al-Delaimy WK. 2018. Acetylcholinesterase activity and time after a peak pesticide-use period among Ecuadorian children. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 91(2):175–184. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29026987/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00420-017-1265-4.
56.
Zarate VR. 2021. Dirección de Registro de Insumos Pecuarios - Agrocalidad. https://www.agrocalidad.gob.ec/direccion-de-registro-de-insumos-pecuarios/ [accessed 14 September 2021].
57.
Ritchie H, Roser M, Rosado P. 2002. Pesticides. https://ourworldindata.org/pesticides [accessed 8 January 2022].
58.
Simcox NJ, Fenske RA, Wolz SA, Lee IC, Kalman DA. 1995. Pesticides in household dust and soil: exposure pathways for children of agricultural families. Environ Health Perspect 103(12):1126–1134. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8747019/, https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.951031126.
59.
World Health Organization. 2008. WHO Child Growth Standards: Training Course on Child Growth Assessment. https://iris.who.int/handle/10665/43601 [accessed 6 January 2022].
60.
Rasmussen AR, Wohlfahrt-Veje C, Tefre de Renzy-Martin K, Hagen CP, Tinggaard J, Mouritsen A, et al. 2015. Validity of self-assessment of pubertal maturation. Pediatrics 135(1):86–93. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25535262/, https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-0793.
61.
Emmanuel M, Bokor BR, Bornstein MH. 2020. Tanner Stages. Treasure Island, FL: StatPearls Publishing.
62.
Marshall WA, Tanner JM. 1969. Variations in pattern of pubertal changes in girls. Arch Dis Child 44(235):291–303. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/5785179/, https://doi.org/10.1136/adc.44.235.291.
63.
Editor's note: Reference and citation removed after initial publication. See erratum https://doi.org/10.1289/EHP14201 for details.
64.
Kemp SL, Korkman M. 2010. Essentials of NEPSY-II Assessment, vol. 69. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
65.
Brooks BL, Sherman EMS, Strauss E. 2009. NEPSY-II: a developmental neuropsychological assessment, second edition. Child Neuropsychol 16(1):80–101, https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040903146966.
66.
Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp S. 2007. NEPSY II: Clinical and Interpretive Manual. 2nd ed. London, UK: Harcourt Assessment.
67.
Suarez-Lopez JR, Himes JH, Jacobs DR, Alexander BH, Gunnar MR. 2013. Acetylcholinesterase activity and neurodevelopment in boys and girls. Pediatrics 132(6):e1649–e1658. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24249815/, https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-0108.
68.
Suarez-Lopez JR, Checkoway H, Jacobs DR, Al-Delaimy WK, Gahagan S. 2017. Potential short-term neurobehavioral alterations in children associated with a peak pesticide spray season: the Mother’s day flower harvest in Ecuador. Neurotoxicology 60(5):125–133. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28188819/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2017.02.002.
69.
Baker SE, Serafim AB, Morales-Agudelo P, Vidal M, Calafat AM, Ospina M. 2019. Quantification of DEET and neonicotinoid pesticide biomarkers in human urine by online solid-phase extraction high-performance liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry. Anal Bioanal Chem 411(3):669–678. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30483854/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00216-018-1481-0.
70.
Fisher RA. 1948. Statistical Tables for Biological, vol. 7. Edinburgh, UK: Oliver and Boyd.
71.
Knuth DE. 1960. A imaginary number system. Commun ACM 3(4):245–247, https://doi.org/10.1145/367177.367233.
72.
Davis MD, Wade EL, Restrepo PR, Roman-Esteva W, Bravo R, Kuklenyik P, et al. 2013. Semi-automated solid phase extraction method for the mass spectrometric quantification of 12 specific metabolites of organophosphorus pesticides, synthetic pyrethroids, and select herbicides in human urine. J Chromatogr B Analyt Technol Biomed Life Sci 929:18–26. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23648311/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jchromb.2013.04.005.
73.
Kwon W, Kim JY, Suh S, In MK. 2012. Simultaneous determination of creatinine and uric acid in urine by liquid chromatography – tandem mass spectrometry with polarity switching electrospray ionization. Forensic Sci Int 221(1–3):57–64. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22503624/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.forsciint.2012.03.025.
74.
Salimetrics. 2016. Salivary Testosterone Protocol. https://salimetrics.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/03/testosterone-saliva-elisa-kit.pdf [accessed 10 April 2020].
75.
Salimetrics. 2019. Salivary Dhea Enzyme Immunoassay Kit. https://salimetrics.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/03/dhea-saliva-elisa-kit.pdf [accessed 10 April 2020].
76.
Salimetrics. 2019. High Sensitivity Salivary 17β-Estradiol Enzyme Immunoassay Kit. https://salimetrics.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/03/17beta-estradiol-saliva-elisa-kit.pdf [accessed 10 April 2020].
77.
Salimetrics. 2006. Salimetrics L.L.C. Expanded Range High Sensitivity Salivary Cortisol Enzyme Immunoassay Kit. https://salimetrics.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/03/salivary-cortisol-elisa-kit.pdf [accessed 10 April 2020].
78.
Wang H, Peng J, Wang B, Lu X, Zheng JZ, Wang K, et al. 2017. Inconsistency between univariate and multiple logistic regressions. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry 29(2):124–128. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28765686/, https://doi.org/10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.217031.
79.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 2021. Fourth National Report on Human Exposure to Environmental Chemicals, Updated Tables, (March 2021). https://www.cdc.gov/exposurereport/ [accessed 10 October 2022].
80.
Schütze A, Morales-Agudelo P, Vidal M, Calafat AM, Ospina M. 2021. Quantification of glyphosate and other organophosphorus compounds in human urine via ion chromatography isotope dilution tandem mass spectrometry. Chemosphere 274:129427. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33529959/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chemosphere.2020.129427.
81.
Hornung RW, Reed LD. 1990. Estimation of average concentration in the presence of nondetectable values estimation of average concentration in the presence of nondetectable values. Appl Occup Environ Hyg 5(1):46–51, https://doi.org/10.1080/1047322X.1990.10389587.
82.
Textor J, van der Zander B, Gilthorpe MS, Liskiewicz M, Ellison GT. 2016. Robust causal inference using directed acyclic graphs: the R package ‘dagitty’. Int J Epidemiol 45(6):1887–1894. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28089956/, https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyw341.
83.
Moses M, Johnson ES, Anger WK, Burse VW, Horstman SW, Jackson RJ, et al. 1993. Environmental equity and pesticide exposure. Toxicol Ind Health 9(5):913–959. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8184449/, https://doi.org/10.1177/074823379300900512.
84.
Jackson E, Shoemaker R, Larian N, Cassis L. 2018. Adipose tissue as a site of toxin accumulation. Compr Physiol 7(4):1085–1135, https://doi.org/10.1002/cphy.c160038.
85.
Carrieri M, Trevisan A, Bartolucci GB. 2001. Adjustment to concentration-dilution of spot urine samples: correlation between specific gravity and creatinine. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 74(1):63–67. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11196084/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s004200000190.
86.
Pivina L, Semenova Y, Doşa MD, Dauletyarova M, Bjørklund G. 2019. Iron deficiency, cognitive functions, and neurobehavioral disorders in children. J Mol Neurosci 68(1):1–10. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30778834/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s12031-019-01276-1.
87.
Ozen S, Goksen D, Darcan S. 2014. Agricultural pesticides and precocious puberty. Vitam Horm 94:27–40, https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800095-3.00002-X.
88.
Peper JS, Dahl RE. 2013. The teenage brain: surging hormones—brain-behavior interactions during puberty. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 22(2):134–139. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26290625/, https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412473755.
89.
Harari R, Julvez J, Murata K, Barr D, Bellinger DC, Debes F, et al. 2010. Neurobehavioral deficits and increased blood pressure in school-age children prenatally exposed to pesticides. Environ Health Perspect 118(6):890–896. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20185383/, https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.0901582.
90.
Elman JA, Jak AJ, Panizzon MS, Tu XM, Chen T, Reynolds CA, et al. 2018. Underdiagnosis of mild cognitive impairment: a consequence of ignoring practice effects. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 10:372–381. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30003138/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2018.04.003.
91.
Field CA, Welsh AH. 2007. Bootstrapping clustered data. J R Stat Soc Ser B Stat Methodol 69(3):369–390, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9868.2007.00593.x.
92.
Gunzler D, Tang W, Lu N, Wu P, Tu XM. 2014. A class of distribution-free models for longitudinal mediation analysis. Psychometrika 79(4):543–568. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24271505/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-013-9355-z.
93.
Bollen KA. 2014. Structural Equations with Latent Variables. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
94.
Rosseel Y. 2012. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Soft 48(2):1–36, https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02.
95.
Klem L. 2000. Structural equation modeling. In: Reading and Understanding MORE Multivariate Statistics. Grimm LG, Yarnold PR, eds. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 227–260.
96.
Jaccard J, Wan C. 2012. LISREL Approaches to Interaction Effects in Multiple Regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
97.
Kline RB. 2004. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
98.
Kumar S, Upadhaya G. 2017. Structure equation modeling basic assumptions and concepts: a novices guide. Int J Quant Qual Res Methods 5(4):10–16.
99.
Richardson JR, Fitsanakis V, Westerink RHS, Kanthasamy AG. 2019. Neurotoxicity of pesticides. Acta Neuropathol 138(3):343–362. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31197504/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00401-019-02033-9.
100.
Zhang D, Wu Y, Yuan Y, Liu W, Kuang H, Yang J, et al. 2017. Exposure to 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid induces oxidative stress and apoptosis in mouse testis. Pestic Biochem Physiol 141:18–22. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28911736/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pestbp.2016.10.006.
101.
Raouf GA, Qusti SY, Ali AM, Dakhakhni TH. 2012. The mechanism of 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid neurotoxicity on rat brain tissue by using FTIR spectroscopy. Life Sci J 9(4):1686–1697.
102.
Redza-Dutordoir M, Averill-Bates DA. 2016. Activation of apoptosis signalling pathways by reactive oxygen species. Biochim Biophys Acta 1863(12):2977–2992. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27646922/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbamcr.2016.09.012.
103.
Ueda RMR, de Souza VM, Magalhães LR, Chagas PHN, Veras ASC, Teixeira GR, et al. 2021. Neurotoxicity associated with chronic exposure to dichlorophenoxyacetic acid (2,4-D)–a simulation of environmental exposure in adult rats. J Environ Sci Health B 56(8):695–705. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/34125002/, https://doi.org/10.1080/03601234.2021.1939622.
104.
Ueda RMR, de Souza VM, Magalhães LR, Giuffrida R, Nai GA. 2021. Alteration of object recognition memory after chronic exposure to dichlorophenoxyacetic acid (2,4-D) in adult rats. RSD 10(1):e23310111695, https://doi.org/10.33448/rsd-v10i1.11695.
105.
Jin Y, Liu Z, Peng T, Fu Z. 2015. The toxicity of chlorpyrifos on the early life stage of zebrafish: a survey on the endpoints at development, locomotor behavior, oxidative stress and immunotoxicity. Fish Shellfish Immunol 43(2):405–414. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25634256/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fsi.2015.01.010.
106.
Richendrfer H, Creton R. 2015. Chlorpyrifos and malathion have opposite effects on behaviors and brain size that are not correlated to changes in AChE activity. Neurotoxicology 49:50–58. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25983063/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2015.05.002.
107.
Loukusa S, Mäkinen L, Kuusikko-Gauffin S, Ebeling H, Moilanen I. 2014. Theory of mind and emotion recognition skills in children with specific language impairment, autism spectrum disorder and typical development: group differences and connection to knowledge of grammatical morphology, word-finding abilities and verbal working memory. Int J Lang Commun Disord 49(4):498–507. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24888967/, https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12091.
108.
Kuusikko S, Haapsamo H, Jansson-Verkasalo E, Hurtig T, Mattila M-L, Ebeling H, et al. 2009. Emotion recognition in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 39(6):938–945. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19205857/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0700-0.
109.
Lemonnier E, Degrez C, Phelep M, Tyzio R, Josse F, Grandgeorge M, et al. 2012. A randomised controlled trial of bumetanide in the treatment of autism in children. Transl Psychiatry 2(12):e202. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23233021/, https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2012.124.
110.
Beecham J, Seneff S. 2015. The possible link between autism and glyphosate acting as glycine Mimetic – a review of evidence from the literature with analysis. J Mol Genet Med 9:1–16, https://doi.org/10.4172/1747-0862.1000187.
111.
Rump KM, Giovannelli JL, Minshew NJ, Strauss MS. 2009. The development of emotion recognition in individuals with autism. Child Dev 80(5):1434–1447. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19765010/, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01343.x.
112.
Palmieri L, Papaleo V, Porcelli V, Scarcia P, Gaita L, Sacco R, et al. 2010. Altered calcium homeostasis in autism-spectrum disorders: evidence from biochemical and genetic studies of the mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate carrier AGC1. Mol Psychiatry 15(1):38–52. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18607376/, https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2008.63.
113.
Nguyen RL, Medvedeva YV, Ayyagari TE, Schmunk G, Gargus JJ. 2018. Intracellular calcium dysregulation in autism spectrum disorder: an analysis of converging organelle signaling pathways. Biochim Biophys Acta Mol Cell Res 1865(11 pt B):1718–1732. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30992134/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbamcr.2018.08.003.
114.
Fucile S, De Saint Jan D, De Carvalho LP, Bregestovski P. 2000. Fast potentiation of glycine receptor channels by intracellular calcium in neurons and transfected cells. Neuron 28(2):571–583. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11144365/, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00134-3.
115.
Panuwet P, Ladva C, Barr DB, Prapamontol T, Meeker JD, D’Souza PE, et al. 2018. Investigation of associations between exposures to pesticides and testosterone levels in Thai farmers. Arch Environ Occup Health 73(4):205–218. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28901838/, https://doi.org/10.1080/19338244.2017.1378606.
116.
Guerrero-Estevez SM, Lopez-Lopez E. 2016. Effects of endocrine disruptors on reproduction in viviparous teleosts with intraluminal gestation. Rev Fish Biol Fisheries 26(3):563–587, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11160-016-9443-0.
117.
Lozano VL, Vinocur A, Sabio y García CA, Allende L, Cristos DS, Rojas D, et al. 2018. Effects of glyphosate and 2,4-D mixture on freshwater phytoplankton and periphyton communities: a microcosms approach. Ecotoxicol Environ Saf 148:1010–1019, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoenv.2017.12.006.
118.
Carvalho WF, Ruiz de Arcaute C, Torres L, de Melo e Silva D, Soloneski S, Larramendy ML. 2020. Genotoxicity of mixtures of glyphosate with 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid chemical forms towards Cnesterodon decemmaculatus (Pisces, Poeciliidae). Environ Sci Pollut Res Int 27(6):6515–6525. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31873893/, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11356-019-07379-x.
119.
Connolly A, Jones K, Basinas I, Galea KS, Kenny L, McGowan P, et al. 2019. Exploring the half-life of glyphosate in human urine samples. Int J Hyg Environ Health 222(2):205–210. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30293930/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijheh.2018.09.004.
120.
Kohli JD, Khanna RN, Gupta BN, Dhar MM, Tandon JS, Sircar KP. 1974. Absorption and excretion of 2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid in man. Xenobiotica 4(2):97–100. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/4828800/, https://doi.org/10.3109/00498257409049349.
121.
Selim S, Hartnagel REJ, Osimitz TG, Gabriel KL, Schoenig GP. 1995. Absorption, metabolism, and excretion of N,N-diethyl-m-toluamide following dermal application to human volunteers. Fundam Appl Toxicol 25(1):95–100. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/7601331/, https://doi.org/10.1006/faat.1995.1043.
122.
Suarez-Lopez JR, Nazeeh N, Kayser G, Suárez-Torres J, Checkoway H, López-Paredes D, et al. 2020. Residential proximity to greenhouse crops and pesticide exposure (via acetylcholinesterase activity) assessed from childhood through adolescence. Environ Res 188:109728. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32798937/, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2020.109728.
123.
Harari R. 2004. Seguridad, salud y ambiente en la floricultura. In: Quito Corporación IFA. https://www.ifa.org.ec/docs/floricultura.pdf [accessed 27 September 2022].
124.
Pinault LL, Hunter FF. 2011. Malaria knowledge, concern, land management, and protection practices among land owners and/or managers in lowland versus highland Ecuador. Malar Res Treat 2011:765125. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22363897/, https://doi.org/10.4061/2011/765125.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Environmental Health Perspectives
Volume 131Issue 10October 2023
PubMed: 37819080

History

Received: 7 April 2022
Revision received: 22 August 2023
Accepted: 30 August 2023
Published online: 11 October 2023
Corrected: 7 December 2023

Authors

Affiliations

The Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
School of Public Health, San Diego State University, San Diego, California, USA
Kun Yang
The Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
Audrey R. Yang
The Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
Tuo Lin
The Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
Xin M. Tu
The Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
Dolores Lopez-Paredes
Fundación Cimas del Ecuador, Quito, Ecuador
Harvey Checkoway
The Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
Jose Suarez-Torres
Fundación Cimas del Ecuador, Quito, Ecuador
Sheila Gahagan
Department of Pediatrics, University of California San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
Danilo Martinez
Fundación Cimas del Ecuador, Quito, Ecuador
Dana Barr
Gangarosa Department of Environmental Health, Rollins School of Public Health, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
Raeanne C. Moore
Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, San Diego, California, USA
The Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California San Diego, San Diego, California, USA

Notes

Address correspondence to Jose R. Suarez-Lopez, The Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive #0725, San Diego, CA 92093-0725 USA. Telephone: (858) 822-0165. Email: [email protected]

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

About Article Metrics


Citations

Download citation

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click DOWNLOAD.

Cited by

  • A Thorny Problem: Glyphosate and 2,4-D Associated with Neurobehavioral Effects for Ecuadorian Adolescents, Environmental Health Perspectives, 10.1289/EHP14169, 131, 12, (2023).

View Options

View options

PDF

View PDF

Get Access

Restore your content access

Enter your email address to restore your content access:

Note: This functionality works only for purchases done as a guest. If you already have an account, log in to access the content to which you are entitled.

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Copy the content Link

Share on social media